Year |
Citation |
Score |
2013 |
Richard L, Waller D. Toward a definition of intrinsic axes: the effect of orthogonality and symmetry on the preferred direction of spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 39: 1914-29. PMID 23751008 DOI: 10.1037/A0032995 |
0.367 |
|
2012 |
Brom C, Vyhnánek J, Lukavský J, Waller D, Kadlec R. A computational model of the allocentric and egocentric spatial memory by means of virtual agents, or how simple virtual agents can help to build complex computational models Cognitive Systems Research. 17: 1-24. DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogsys.2011.09.001 |
0.306 |
|
2011 |
Friedman A, Waller D, Thrash T, Greenauer N, Hodgson E. View combination: a generalization mechanism for visual recognition. Cognition. 119: 229-41. PMID 21334606 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2011.01.012 |
0.318 |
|
2010 |
Greenauer N, Waller D. Micro- and macroreference frames: Specifying the relations between spatial categories in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 36: 938-57. PMID 20565211 DOI: 10.1037/A0019647 |
0.353 |
|
2010 |
Mello C, Waller D. Monitoring object orientation: Effects of layout complexity, viewpoint changes, and object function Spatial Cognition and Computation. 10: 313-340. DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2010.487963 |
0.336 |
|
2009 |
Waller D, Friedman A, Hodgson E, Greenauer N. Learning scenes from multiple views: novel views can be recognized more efficiently than learned views. Memory & Cognition. 37: 90-9. PMID 19103979 DOI: 10.3758/Mc.37.1.90 |
0.339 |
|
2008 |
Greenauer N, Waller D. Intrinsic array structure is neither necessary nor sufficient for nonegocentric coding of spatial layouts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 15: 1015-21. PMID 18926998 DOI: 10.3758/Pbr.15.5.1015 |
0.36 |
|
2008 |
Friedman A, Waller D. View combination in scene recognition. Memory & Cognition. 36: 467-78. PMID 18491488 DOI: 10.3758/Mc.36.3.467 |
0.34 |
|
2008 |
Waller D, Lippa Y, Richardson A. Isolating observer-based reference directions in human spatial memory: head, body, and the self-to-array axis. Cognition. 106: 157-83. PMID 17316594 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2007.01.002 |
0.371 |
|
2007 |
Waller D, Bachmann E, Hodgson E, Beall AC. The HIVE: a huge immersive virtual environment for research in spatial cognition. Behavior Research Methods. 39: 835-43. PMID 18183898 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03192976 |
0.31 |
|
2007 |
Waller D, Lippa Y. Landmarks as beacons and associative cues: their role in route learning. Memory & Cognition. 35: 910-24. PMID 17910176 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193465 |
0.32 |
|
2007 |
Waller D, Greenauer N. The role of body-based sensory information in the acquisition of enduring spatial representations. Psychological Research. 71: 322-32. PMID 16953434 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-006-0087-X |
0.359 |
|
2006 |
Waller D. Egocentric and nonegocentric coding in memory for spatial layout: evidence from scene recognition. Memory & Cognition. 34: 491-504. PMID 16933759 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193573 |
0.362 |
|
2006 |
Waller D, Hodgson E. Transient and enduring spatial representations under disorientation and self-rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 32: 867-82. PMID 16822154 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.4.867 |
0.361 |
|
2006 |
Hodgson E, Waller D. Lack of set size effects in spatial updating: Evidence for offline updating. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 32: 854-66. PMID 16822153 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.4.854 |
0.369 |
|
2005 |
Waller D. The WALKABOUT: Using virtual environments to assess large-scale spatial abilities Computers in Human Behavior. 21: 243-253. DOI: 10.1016/J.Chb.2004.02.022 |
0.335 |
|
2004 |
Waller D, Loomis JM, Haun DB. Body-based senses enhance knowledge of directions in large-scale environments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 11: 157-63. PMID 15117002 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03206476 |
0.332 |
|
2004 |
Waller D, Beall AC, Loomis JM. Using virtual environments to assess directional knowledge Journal of Environmental Psychology. 24: 105-116. DOI: 10.1016/S0272-4944(03)00051-3 |
0.314 |
|
2004 |
Hegarty M, Waller D. A dissociation between mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abilities Intelligence. 32: 175-191. DOI: 10.1016/J.Intell.2003.12.001 |
0.563 |
|
2003 |
Waller D, Loomis JM, Steck SD. Inertial cues do not enhance knowledge of environmental layout. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 10: 987-93. PMID 15000550 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03196563 |
0.35 |
|
2003 |
Waller D, Haun DB. Scaling techniques for modeling directional knowledge. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers : a Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc. 35: 285-93. PMID 12834086 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03202554 |
0.319 |
|
2002 |
Waller D, Montello DR, Richardson AE, Hegarty M. Orientation specificity and spatial updating of memories for layouts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 28: 1051-63. PMID 12450331 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.28.6.1051 |
0.639 |
|
2001 |
Waller D, Knapp D, Hunt E. Spatial representations of virtual mazes: the role of visual fidelity and individual differences. Human Factors. 43: 147-58. PMID 11474760 DOI: 10.1518/001872001775992561 |
0.375 |
|
2000 |
Waller D. Individual differences in spatial learning from computer-simulated environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied. 6: 307-21. PMID 11218340 DOI: 10.1037/1076-898X.6.4.307 |
0.371 |
|
2000 |
Waller D, Loomis JM, Golledge RG, Beall AC. Spatial Cognition and Computation. 2: 333-354. DOI: 10.1023/A:1015514424931 |
0.326 |
|
1999 |
Waller D. Factors affecting the perception of interobject distances in virtual environments Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments. 8: 657-670. DOI: 10.1162/105474699566549 |
0.322 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |