Year |
Citation |
Score |
2024 |
Kershner AM, Hollingworth A. Are selection history effects limited to implicit forms of memory? Evidence from intertrial repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. PMID 39264673 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001387 |
0.382 |
|
2024 |
Paavola ML, Hollingworth A, Moore CM. Saccades to partially occluded objects: Perceptual completion mediates oculomotor control. Journal of Vision. 24: 8. PMID 38546587 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.3.8 |
0.625 |
|
2023 |
Kershner AM, Hollingworth A. Category-specific learning of color, orientation, and position regularities guide visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 49: 907-922. PMID 37276127 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0001098 |
0.404 |
|
2022 |
Kershner AM, Hollingworth A. Real-world object categories and scene contexts conjointly structure statistical learning for the guidance of visual search. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 84: 1304-1316. PMID 35426031 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-022-02475-6 |
0.427 |
|
2021 |
Thayer DD, Bahle B, Hollingworth A. Guidance of attention from visual working memory is feature-based, not object-based: Implications for models of feature binding. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. PMID 34735186 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001116 |
0.519 |
|
2021 |
Bahle B, Kershner AM, Hollingworth A. Categorical cuing: Object categories structure the acquisition of statistical regularities to guide visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 150: 2552-2566. PMID 33829823 DOI: 10.1037/xge0001059 |
0.406 |
|
2021 |
Tas AC, Mordkoff JT, Hollingworth A. Object-mediated overwriting across saccades. Journal of Vision. 21: 3. PMID 33538771 DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.2.3 |
0.378 |
|
2021 |
Hein E, Stepper MY, Hollingworth A, Moore CM. Visual working memory content influences correspondence processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 33507771 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000890 |
0.581 |
|
2019 |
Bahle B, Thayer DD, Mordkoff JT, Hollingworth A. The architecture of working memory: Features from multiple remembered objects produce parallel, coactive guidance of attention in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 149: 967-983. PMID 31589068 DOI: 10.1037/xge0000694 |
0.519 |
|
2019 |
Hollingworth A, Bahle B. Feature-based guidance of attention by visual working memory is applied independently of remembered object location. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 31140137 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-019-01759-8 |
0.606 |
|
2019 |
Hollingworth A, Matsukura M. Feature-based guidance of attention during post-saccadic selection. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 30980343 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-019-01719-2 |
0.781 |
|
2019 |
Bahle B, Hollingworth A. Contrasting episodic and template-based guidance during search through natural scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 45: 523-536. PMID 30920285 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000624 |
0.573 |
|
2019 |
Bahle B, Hollingworth A. Visual search for categorical targets is biased toward recently viewed exemplars Journal of Vision. 19: 254b. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.254b |
0.463 |
|
2019 |
Hollingworth A, Bahle B, Thayer D, Mordkoff JT. The Architecture of Interaction between Visual Working Memory and Attention: Features from Multiple Remembered Objects Produce Parallel, Coactive Guidance Journal of Vision. 19: 213c. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.213c |
0.573 |
|
2018 |
Van der Stigchel S, Hollingworth A. Visuospatial Working Memory as a Fundamental Component of the Eye Movement System. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 27: 136-143. PMID 29805202 DOI: 10.1177/0963721417741710 |
0.512 |
|
2018 |
Bahle B, Beck VM, Hollingworth A. The architecture of interaction between visual working memory and visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 29629781 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000509 |
0.813 |
|
2017 |
Beck VM, Luck SJ, Hollingworth A. Whatever You Do, Don't Look at the . . .: Evaluating Guidance by an Exclusionary Attentional Template. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 29035075 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000485 |
0.84 |
|
2017 |
Bahle B, Matsukura M, Hollingworth A. Contrasting Gist-Based and Template-Based Guidance During Real-World Visual Search. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 28795834 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000468 |
0.796 |
|
2017 |
Beck VM, Hollingworth A. Competition in saccade target selection reveals attentional guidance by simultaneously active working memory representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 43: 225-230. PMID 28134550 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000306 |
0.785 |
|
2017 |
Matsukura M, Hollingworth A. Erratum to: Does visual short-term memory have a high-capacity stage? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. PMID 28120315 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-017-1228-5 |
0.736 |
|
2016 |
Hollingworth A, Beck VM. Memory-Based Attention Capture When Multiple Items Are Maintained in Visual Working Memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 27123681 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000230 |
0.82 |
|
2016 |
Tas AC, Luck SJ, Hollingworth A. The Relationship Between Visual Attention and Visual Working Memory Encoding: A Dissociation Between Covert and Overt Orienting. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 26854532 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000212 |
0.736 |
|
2016 |
Bahle B, Hollingworth A. Contrasting Gist-based and Feature-based Guidance during Real-world Search Journal of Vision. 16: 347. DOI: 10.1167/16.12.347 |
0.328 |
|
2016 |
Beck V, Leonard C, Robinson B, Hahn B, Hollingworth A, Gold J, Luck S. People with Schizophrenia Demonstrate More Optimal Feature-Guided Visual Search in a Probabilistic Search Task Journal of Vision. 16: 338. DOI: 10.1167/16.12.338 |
0.774 |
|
2015 |
Hollingworth A, Beck V. Guidance of Attention by Multiple Feature Values in Visual Working Memory. Journal of Vision. 15: 1363. PMID 26327051 DOI: 10.1167/15.12.1363 |
0.802 |
|
2015 |
Beck VM, Hollingworth A. Evidence for negative feature guidance in visual search is explained by spatial recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 41: 1190-6. PMID 26191616 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000109 |
0.813 |
|
2015 |
Hollingworth A. Visual working memory modulates within-object metrics of saccade landing position. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1339: 11-9. PMID 25684544 DOI: 10.1111/nyas.12664 |
0.596 |
|
2014 |
Tas AC, Moore CM, Hollingworth A. The representation of the saccade target object depends on visual stability. Visual Cognition. 22: 1042-1046. PMID 25544827 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2014.960671 |
0.679 |
|
2014 |
Schneegans S, Spencer JP, Schöner G, Hwang S, Hollingworth A. Dynamic interactions between visual working memory and saccade target selection. Journal of Vision. 14. PMID 25228628 DOI: 10.1167/14.11.9 |
0.584 |
|
2014 |
Luck SJ, McClenon C, Beck VM, Hollingworth A, Leonard CJ, Hahn B, Robinson BM, Gold JM. Hyperfocusing in schizophrenia: Evidence from interactions between working memory and eye movements. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 123: 783-95. PMID 25089655 DOI: 10.1037/Abn0000003 |
0.801 |
|
2014 |
Tas C, Moore C, Hollingworth A. The role of visual stability on the representation of saccade target object Journal of Vision. 14: 1231-1231. DOI: 10.1167/14.10.1231 |
0.819 |
|
2014 |
Beck V, Hollingworth A. Can Attention be Guided Efficiently by a Negative Template? Journal of Vision. 14: 1050-1050. DOI: 10.1167/14.10.1050 |
0.733 |
|
2013 |
Hollingworth A, Matsukura M, Luck SJ. Visual working memory modulates low-level saccade target selection: evidence from rapidly generated saccades in the global effect paradigm. Journal of Vision. 13: 4. PMID 24190909 DOI: 10.1167/13.13.4 |
0.833 |
|
2013 |
Hollingworth A, Hwang S. The relationship between visual working memory and attention: retention of precise colour information in the absence of effects on perceptual selection. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 368: 20130061. PMID 24018723 DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0061 |
0.524 |
|
2013 |
Hollingworth A, Matsukura M, Luck SJ. Visual working memory modulates rapid eye movements to simple onset targets. Psychological Science. 24: 790-6. PMID 23508739 DOI: 10.1177/0956797612459767 |
0.829 |
|
2013 |
Hollingworth A, Maxcey-Richard AM. Selective maintenance in visual working memory does not require sustained visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 39: 1047-58. PMID 23067118 DOI: 10.1037/a0030238 |
0.557 |
|
2013 |
Maxcey-Richard AM, Hollingworth A. The strategic retention of task-relevant objects in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 39: 760-72. PMID 22845068 DOI: 10.1037/a0029496 |
0.539 |
|
2013 |
Hein E, Hollingworth A, Moore CM. The influence of visual working memory on correspondence in the Ternus display Journal of Vision. 13: 61-61. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.61 |
0.631 |
|
2013 |
Beck V, Hollingworth A. Template-based guidance in visual search is independent of influence from properties of currently or recently fixated objects Journal of Vision. 13: 532-532. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.532 |
0.789 |
|
2013 |
Hollingworth A, Hwang S, Luck SJ. The Influence of Saccade Execution on Spatial Working Memory Precision Journal of Vision. 13: 1351-1351. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.1351 |
0.616 |
|
2012 |
Hollingworth A. Guidance of visual search by memory and knowledge. Nebraska Symposium On Motivation. Nebraska Symposium On Motivation. 59: 63-89. PMID 23437630 DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4614-4794-8-4 |
0.552 |
|
2012 |
Hollingworth A. Task specificity and the influence of memory on visual search: comment on Võ and Wolfe (2012). Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 38: 1596-603. PMID 23205947 DOI: 10.1037/a0030237 |
0.564 |
|
2012 |
Tas AC, Moore CM, Hollingworth A. An object-mediated updating account of insensitivity to transsaccadic change. Journal of Vision. 12: 18. PMID 23092946 DOI: 10.1167/12.11.18 |
0.692 |
|
2012 |
Tas AC, Dodd MD, Hollingworth A. The Role of Surface Feature Continuity in Object-based Inhibition of Return. Visual Cognition. 20: 29-47. PMID 23082073 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2011.626466 |
0.489 |
|
2012 |
Beck VM, Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. Simultaneous control of attention by multiple working memory representations. Psychological Science. 23: 887-98. PMID 22760886 DOI: 10.1177/0956797612439068 |
0.844 |
|
2012 |
Hahn B, Hollingworth A, Robinson BM, Kaiser ST, Leonard CJ, Beck VM, Kappenman ES, Luck SJ, Gold JM. Control of working memory content in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 134: 70-5. PMID 22079944 DOI: 10.1016/J.Schres.2011.10.008 |
0.772 |
|
2012 |
Wisco BE, Treat TA, Hollingworth A. Visual attention to emotion in depression: facilitation and withdrawal processes. Cognition & Emotion. 26: 602-14. PMID 21851151 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2011.595392 |
0.368 |
|
2012 |
Hollingworth A, Maxcey-Richard AM, Vecera SP. The spatial distribution of attention within and across objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 38: 135-51. PMID 21728455 DOI: 10.1037/a0024463 |
0.775 |
|
2012 |
Tas CA, Luck SJ, Hollingworth A. Saccade execution, not covert attention, leads to automatic encoding of distractors into VWM Journal of Vision. 12: 852-852. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.852 |
0.806 |
|
2012 |
Hwang S, Hollingworth A. The Reliance on Ensemble Statistics in VWM Varies According to the Quality of Item Memory Journal of Vision. 12: 352-352. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.352 |
0.349 |
|
2011 |
Matsukura M, Hollingworth A. Does visual short-term memory have a high-capacity stage? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 18: 1098-104. PMID 21935737 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-011-0153-2 |
0.742 |
|
2011 |
Mills M, Hollingworth A, Van der Stigchel S, Hoffman L, Dodd MD. Examining the influence of task set on eye movements and fixations. Journal of Vision. 11: 17. PMID 21799023 DOI: 10.1167/11.8.17 |
0.416 |
|
2011 |
Beck V, Luck S, Hollingworth A. The implementation of an exclusionary attentional template: Effects of cue-stimulus delay, predictability of relevant features, and direct versus indirect cueing F1000research. 2. DOI: 10.7490/F1000Research.1564.1 |
0.779 |
|
2011 |
Tas C, Moore C, Hollingworth A. The role of surface feature information in object persistence across saccades Journal of Vision. 11: 519-519. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.519 |
0.796 |
|
2011 |
Matsukura M, Hollingworth A. How does object structure influence saccade targeting within an object? Journal of Vision. 11: 487-487. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.487 |
0.775 |
|
2011 |
Dodd M, Mills M, Van der Stigchel S, Hollingworth A. Examining the influence of scene manipulations and task instruction on scanpaths and inhibition of return Journal of Vision. 11: 481-481. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.481 |
0.316 |
|
2011 |
Beck V, Luck S, Hollingworth A. The Implementation of an Exclusionary Attentional Template: Direct Versus Indirect Cueing Journal of Vision. 11: 1309-1309. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.1309 |
0.785 |
|
2011 |
Caglar Tas A, Luck SJ, Hollingworth A. The automatic encoding of distractors into visual working memory through overt, but not covert attention Visual Cognition. 19: 1331-1335. |
0.649 |
|
2010 |
Brooks DI, Rasmussen IP, Hollingworth A. The nesting of search contexts within natural scenes: evidence from contextual cuing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 36: 1406-18. PMID 20731525 DOI: 10.1037/a0019257 |
0.799 |
|
2010 |
Hollingworth A, Simons DJ, Franconeri SL. New objects do not capture attention without a sensory transient. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 72: 1298-310. PMID 20601711 DOI: 10.3758/APP.72.5.1298 |
0.493 |
|
2010 |
Hollingworth A, Rasmussen IP. Binding objects to locations: the relationship between object files and visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 36: 543-64. PMID 20515188 DOI: 10.1037/a0017836 |
0.831 |
|
2010 |
Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. The role of visual working memory in establishing object correspondence across saccades Journal of Vision. 9: 414-414. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.414 |
0.714 |
|
2010 |
Brooks DI, Rasmussen IP, Hollingworth A. The interaction between global and local scene features in contextual cueing Journal of Vision. 8: 869-869. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.869 |
0.787 |
|
2010 |
Rasmussen IP, Hollingworth A. The capacity for spatial updating in visual short-term memory Journal of Vision. 8: 211-211. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.211 |
0.809 |
|
2010 |
Richard AM, Hollingworth A. Strategic control of visual short-term memory during scene viewing Journal of Vision. 8: 204-204. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.204 |
0.769 |
|
2010 |
Dodd M, Van Der Stigchel S, Hollingworth A, Kingstone A. Examining scanpaths and inhibition of return as a function of task instruction during scene viewing Journal of Vision. 8: 118-118. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.118 |
0.311 |
|
2010 |
Hollingworth A, Rasmussen IP. The binding of objects to locations in visual short-term memory Journal of Vision. 7: 353-353. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.353 |
0.821 |
|
2010 |
Richard AM, Vecera SP, Hollingworth A. The role of bbject discontinuity in object-based selection Journal of Vision. 7: 1074-1074. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.1074 |
0.82 |
|
2010 |
Hyun J, Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. How change-detection is related to visual search: A change in a remembered object is like a simple feature Journal of Vision. 6: 985-985. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.985 |
0.59 |
|
2010 |
Richard AM, Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. Testing an object file theory of object correspondence across saccades Journal of Vision. 6: 486-486. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.486 |
0.8 |
|
2010 |
Hollingworth A, Sacks DL. The updating of object-position binding in visual short-term memory Journal of Vision. 6: 25-25. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.25 |
0.56 |
|
2010 |
Lin P, Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. Similarity does not produce interference between visual working memory representations Journal of Vision. 5: 616-616. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.616 |
0.65 |
|
2010 |
Sacks DL, Hollingworth A. Attending to original object location facilitates visual memory retrieval Journal of Vision. 5: 443-443. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.443 |
0.582 |
|
2010 |
Johnson JS, Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. The role of attention in binding features in visual working memory Journal of Vision. 5: 426-426. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.426 |
0.672 |
|
2010 |
Matsukura M, Luck S, Hollingworth A. Which Features of an Object are Stored in Visual Working Memory across a Saccade? Evidence from Visual Search Journal of Vision. 10: 724-724. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.724 |
0.821 |
|
2010 |
Hollingworth A, Matsukura M, Luck SJ. Visual Working Memory Influences the Speed and Accuracy of Simple Saccadic Eye Movements Journal of Vision. 10: 550-550. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.550 |
0.792 |
|
2010 |
Spencer J, Schneegans S, Hollingworth A. Dynamic interactions between visual working memory and saccade planning Journal of Vision. 10: 537-537. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.537 |
0.476 |
|
2010 |
Chen H, Anderson DE, Hollingworth A, Vecera S, Moore CM. Visual Working Memory Content Modulates Competition inBinocular Rivalry Journal of Vision. 10: 352-352. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.352 |
0.793 |
|
2010 |
Tas C, Dodd M, Hollingworth A. The role of surface feature and spatiotemporal continuity in object-based inhibition of return Journal of Vision. 10: 178-178. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.178 |
0.783 |
|
2009 |
Hollingworth A, Franconeri SL. Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues. Cognition. 113: 150-66. PMID 19729155 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004 |
0.51 |
|
2009 |
Hyun JS, Woodman GF, Vogel EK, Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. The comparison of visual working memory representations with perceptual inputs. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 35: 1140-60. PMID 19653755 DOI: 10.1037/a0015019 |
0.696 |
|
2009 |
Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. The role of visual working memory (VWM) in the control of gaze during visual search. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 71: 936-49. PMID 19429970 DOI: 10.3758/APP.71.4.936 |
0.727 |
|
2009 |
Dodd MD, Van der Stigchel S, Hollingworth A. Novelty is not always the best policy: inhibition of return and facilitation of return as a function of visual task. Psychological Science. 20: 333-9. PMID 19222812 DOI: 10.1111/J.1467-9280.2009.02294.X |
0.504 |
|
2009 |
Hollingworth A. Two forms of scene memory guide visual search: Memory for scene context and memory for the binding of target object to scene location Visual Cognition. 17: 273-291. DOI: 10.1080/13506280802193367 |
0.576 |
|
2008 |
Richard AM, Luck SJ, Hollingworth A. Establishing object correspondence across eye movements: Flexible use of spatiotemporal and surface feature information. Cognition. 109: 66-88. PMID 18760406 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2008.07.004 |
0.795 |
|
2008 |
Johnson JS, Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. The role of attention in the maintenance of feature bindings in visual short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 34: 41-55. PMID 18248139 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.41 |
0.668 |
|
2008 |
Hollingworth A, Richard AM, Luck SJ. Understanding the function of visual short-term memory: transsaccadic memory, object correspondence, and gaze correction. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 137: 163-81. PMID 18248135 DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.163 |
0.803 |
|
2008 |
Hollingworth A. Visual Memory for Natural Scenes Visual Memory. DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195305487.003.0005 |
0.406 |
|
2008 |
Hollingworth A, Luck SJ. Visual Memory Systems Visual Memory. DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195305487.003.0001 |
0.57 |
|
2008 |
Weierich MR, Treat TA, Hollingworth A. Theories and measurement of visual attentional processing in anxiety Cognition and Emotion. 22: 985-1018. DOI: 10.1080/02699930701597601 |
0.364 |
|
2007 |
Hollingworth A. Object-position binding in visual memory for natural scenes and object arrays. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 33: 31-47. PMID 17311477 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.31 |
0.572 |
|
2006 |
Hollingworth A. Scene and position specificity in visual memory for objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 32: 58-69. PMID 16478340 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.58 |
0.586 |
|
2006 |
Henderson JM, Hollingworth A. Eye Movements, Visual Memory, and Scene Representation Perception of Faces, Objects, and Scenes: Analytic and Holistic Processes. DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195313659.003.0014 |
0.661 |
|
2006 |
Hollingworth A. Visual memory for natural scenes: Evidence from change detection and visual search Visual Cognition. 14: 781-807. DOI: 10.1080/13506280500193818 |
0.564 |
|
2005 |
Hollingworth A, Hyun JS, Zhang W. The role of visual short-term memory in empty cell localization. Perception & Psychophysics. 67: 1332-43. PMID 16555585 DOI: 10.3758/BF03193638 |
0.4 |
|
2005 |
Hollingworth A. The relationship between online visual representation of a scene and long-term scene memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 31: 396-411. PMID 15910127 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.3.396 |
0.504 |
|
2005 |
Franconeri SL, Hollingworth A, Simons DJ. Do new objects capture attention? Psychological Science. 16: 275-81. PMID 15828974 DOI: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01528.x |
0.54 |
|
2005 |
Hollingworth A. Preserved memory for scene brightness following an undetected change Journal of Vision. 5: 916-916. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.916 |
0.413 |
|
2005 |
Richard AM, Hollingworth A, Vecera SP. The spatial distribution of object-based attention Journal of Vision. 5: 1037-1037. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.1037 |
0.817 |
|
2005 |
Hollingworth A. Memory for object position in natural scenes Visual Cognition. 12: 1003-1016. DOI: 10.1080/13506280444000625 |
0.478 |
|
2004 |
Hollingworth A, Henderson JM. Sustained change blindness to incremental scene rotation: a dissociation between explicit change detection and visual memory. Perception & Psychophysics. 66: 800-7. PMID 15495905 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03194974 |
0.647 |
|
2004 |
Hollingworth A, Hollingworth A. Constructing visual representations of natural scenes: the roles of short- and long-term visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 30: 519-37. PMID 15161384 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.3.519 |
0.595 |
|
2004 |
Hollingworth A. Memory guides search in natural scenes Journal of Vision. 4: 177-177. DOI: 10.1167/4.8.177 |
0.454 |
|
2003 |
Hollingworth A, Henderson JM. Testing a conceptual locus for the inconsistent object change detection advantage in real-world scenes. Memory & Cognition. 31: 930-40. PMID 14651300 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03196446 |
0.711 |
|
2003 |
Henderson JM, Hollingworth A. Global transsaccadic change blindness during scene perception. Psychological Science. 14: 493-7. PMID 12930482 DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.02459 |
0.66 |
|
2003 |
Hollingworth A. Failures of retrieval and comparison constrain change detection in natural scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 29: 388-403. PMID 12760623 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.388 |
0.545 |
|
2003 |
Henderson JM, Hollingworth A. Eye movements and visual memory: detecting changes to saccade targets in scenes. Perception & Psychophysics. 65: 58-71. PMID 12699309 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03194783 |
0.71 |
|
2003 |
Hollingworth A. Short- and long-term memory contributions to the online visual representation of natural scenes Journal of Vision. 3: 235a. DOI: 10.1167/3.9.235 |
0.463 |
|
2002 |
Hollingworth A, Henderson JM. Sustained insensitivity to incremental scene rotation: A dissociation between explicit change detection and visual memory Journal of Vision. 2: 251a. DOI: 10.1167/2.7.251 |
0.68 |
|
2002 |
Hollingworth A, Henderson JM. Accurate visual memory for previously attended objects in natural scenes Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 28: 113-136. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.28.1.113 |
0.745 |
|
2001 |
Hollingworth A, Williams CC, Henderson JM. To see and remember: visually specific information is retained in memory from previously attended objects in natural scenes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 8: 761-8. PMID 11848597 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03196215 |
0.757 |
|
2001 |
Hollingworth A, Schrock G, Henderson JM. Change detection in the flicker paradigm: the role of fixation position within the scene. Memory & Cognition. 29: 296-304. PMID 11352212 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03194923 |
0.671 |
|
2000 |
Hollingworth A, Henderson JM. Semantic informativeness mediates the detection of changes in natural scenes Visual Cognition. 7: 213-235. DOI: 10.1080/135062800394775 |
0.686 |
|
1999 |
Hollingworth A, Henderson JM. Object identification is isolated from scene semantic constraint: evidence from object type and token discrimination. Acta Psychologica. 102: 319-43. PMID 10504886 DOI: 10.1016/S0001-6918(98)00053-5 |
0.678 |
|
1999 |
Henderson JM, Hollingworth A. High-level scene perception. Annual Review of Psychology. 50: 243-71. PMID 10074679 DOI: 10.1146/Annurev.Psych.50.1.243 |
0.675 |
|
1999 |
Henderson JM, Hollingworth A. The role of fixation position in detecting scene changes across saccades Psychological Science. 10: 438-443. DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00183 |
0.668 |
|
1999 |
Henderson JM, Weeks PA, Hollingworth A. The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 25: 210-228. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.1.210 |
0.593 |
|
1999 |
Hollingworth A, Henderson JM. Vision and cognition: Drawing the line Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 22: 380-381. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X99382029 |
0.623 |
|
1998 |
Hollingworth A, Henderson JM. Does consistent scene context facilitate object perception? Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 127: 398-415. PMID 9857494 DOI: 10.1037//0096-3445.127.4.398 |
0.664 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |