Year |
Citation |
Score |
2021 |
Luck SJ, Gaspelin N, Folk CL, Remington RW, Theeuwes J. Progress Toward Resolving the Attentional Capture Debate. Visual Cognition. 29: 1-21. PMID 33574729 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2020.1848949 |
0.729 |
|
2020 |
Becker SI, Manoharan RT, Folk CL. The attentional blink: A relational accountof attentional engagement. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. PMID 32989720 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-020-01813-9 |
0.508 |
|
2019 |
Becker SI, Atalla M, Folk CL. Conjunction search: Can we simultaneously bias attention to features and relations? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 31317396 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-019-01807-3 |
0.532 |
|
2019 |
Wyble B, Hess M, Callahan-Flintoft C, Folk C. Rapid covert visual attention to conceptual targets Journal of Vision. 19: 101a. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.101a |
0.484 |
|
2016 |
Anderson BA, Folk CL, Courtney SM. Neural mechanisms of goal-contingent task disengagement: Response-irrelevant stimuli activate the default mode network. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 81: 221-230. PMID 27253724 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cortex.2016.05.006 |
0.599 |
|
2016 |
Anderson BA, Folk CL, Garrison R, Rogers L. Mechanisms of Habitual Approach: Failure to Suppress Irrelevant Responses Evoked by Previously Reward-Associated Stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. PMID 27054684 DOI: 10.1037/Xge0000169 |
0.593 |
|
2015 |
Folk CL, Remington RW. Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 41: 1153-65. PMID 26030438 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000084 |
0.619 |
|
2015 |
Folk C, Folk C. The Influence of Salience on Attentional Capture by Set-Consistent and Set-Inconsistent Stimuli Journal of Vision. 15: 316. DOI: 10.1167/15.12.316 |
0.461 |
|
2014 |
Wu SC, Remington RW, Folk CL. Onsets do not override top-down goals, but they are responded to more quickly. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 76: 649-54. PMID 24596080 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-014-0637-Z |
0.605 |
|
2014 |
Anderson BA, Folk CL. Conditional automaticity in response selection: contingent involuntary response inhibition with varied stimulus-response mapping. Psychological Science. 25: 547-54. PMID 24357615 DOI: 10.1177/0956797613511086 |
0.52 |
|
2014 |
Folk C, Berenato A, Wyble B. Semantic priming produces contingent attentional capture by conceptual content F1000research. 14: 318-318. DOI: 10.7490/F1000Research.1095912.1 |
0.469 |
|
2013 |
Folk CL. Dissociating compatibility effects and distractor costs in the additional singleton paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 434. PMID 23882241 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00434 |
0.501 |
|
2013 |
Becker SI, Folk CL, Remington RW. Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science. 24: 634-47. PMID 23558547 DOI: 10.1177/0956797612458528 |
0.56 |
|
2013 |
Wyble B, Folk C, Potter MC. Contingent attentional capture by conceptually relevant images. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 39: 861-71. PMID 23163786 DOI: 10.1037/A0030517 |
0.572 |
|
2013 |
Folk C, Anderson B. Involuntary Inhibition of Motor Responses Contingent on Top-Down Goals Journal of Vision. 13: 1125-1125. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.1125 |
0.446 |
|
2012 |
Anderson BA, Folk CL. Contingent involuntary motoric inhibition: the involuntary inhibition of a motor response contingent on top-down goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 38: 1348-52. PMID 23106373 DOI: 10.1037/A0030514 |
0.507 |
|
2012 |
Anderson BA, Folk CL. Dissociating location-specific inhibition and attention shifts: evidence against the disengagement account of contingent capture. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 74: 1183-98. PMID 22673857 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-012-0325-9 |
0.717 |
|
2012 |
Irons JL, Folk CL, Remington RW. All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 38: 758-75. PMID 22201470 DOI: 10.1037/A0026578 |
0.574 |
|
2012 |
Wyble B, Folk C, Potter M. Attentional capture by images that match a conceptual target set Journal of Vision. 12: 939-939. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.939 |
0.452 |
|
2010 |
Becker SI, Folk CL, Remington RW. The role of relational information in contingent capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 36: 1460-76. PMID 20919781 DOI: 10.1037/A0020370 |
0.598 |
|
2010 |
Folk CL, Anderson BA. Target-uncertainty effects in attentional capture: color-singleton set or multiple attentional control settings? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 17: 421-6. PMID 20551369 DOI: 10.3758/Pbr.17.3.421 |
0.677 |
|
2010 |
Folk CL, Remington R. A critical evaluation of the disengagement hypothesis. Acta Psychologica. 135: 103-5; discussion 13. PMID 20510849 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2010.04.012 |
0.649 |
|
2010 |
Anderson BA, Folk CL. Variations in the magnitude of attentional capture: testing a two-process model. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 72: 342-52. PMID 20139450 DOI: 10.3758/App.72.2.342 |
0.715 |
|
2010 |
Folk C, Leber A, Egeth H. A blip in the blink: Novel distractors produce sparing at lag 2, but not lag 1 Journal of Vision. 9: 160-160. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.160 |
0.745 |
|
2009 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Wu SC. Additivity of abrupt onset effects supports nonspatial distraction, not the capture of spatial attention. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 71: 308-13. PMID 19304620 DOI: 10.3758/App.71.2.308 |
0.525 |
|
2009 |
Folk CL, Ester EF, Troemel K. How to keep attention from straying: get engaged! Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 16: 127-32. PMID 19145022 DOI: 10.3758/PBR.16.1.127 |
0.767 |
|
2008 |
Folk CL, Remington RW. Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings Visual Cognition. 16: 215-231. DOI: 10.1080/13506280701458804 |
0.584 |
|
2008 |
Folk CL, Leber AB, Egeth HE. Top-down control settings and the attentional blink: Evidence for nonspatial contingent capture Visual Cognition. 16: 616-642. DOI: 10.1080/13506280601134018 |
0.804 |
|
2006 |
Folk CL, Remington R. Top-down modulation of preattentive processing: Testing the recovery account of contingent capture Visual Cognition. 14: 445-465. DOI: 10.1080/13506280500193545 |
0.578 |
|
2002 |
Folk CL, Leber AB, Egeth HE. Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink. Perception & Psychophysics. 64: 741-53. PMID 12201333 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03194741 |
0.812 |
|
2001 |
Remington RW, Folk CL. A dissociation between attention and selection. Psychological Science. 12: 511-5. PMID 11760140 DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00394 |
0.583 |
|
2001 |
Remington RW, Folk CL, McLean JP. Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention? Perception & Psychophysics. 63: 298-307. PMID 11281104 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03194470 |
0.611 |
|
2001 |
Egeth HE, Folk CL, Leber AB, Nakama T, Hendel SK. 5 Attentional capture in the spatial and temporal domains Advances in Psychology. 133: 93-119. DOI: 10.1016/S0166-4115(01)80007-8 |
0.758 |
|
1999 |
Folk CL, Remington R. Can new objects override attentional control settings? Perception & Psychophysics. 61: 727-39. PMID 10370339 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03205541 |
0.576 |
|
1998 |
Folk CL, Remington R. Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 24: 847-58. PMID 9627420 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.3.847 |
0.634 |
|
1997 |
Lincourt AE, Folk CL, Hoyer WJ. Effects of aging on voluntary and involuntary shifts of attention. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition. 4: 290-303. PMID 29053089 DOI: 10.1080/13825589708256654 |
0.495 |
|
1996 |
Ghirardelli TG, Folk CL. Spatial cuing in a stereoscopic display: Evidence for a "depth-blind" attentional spotlight. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 3: 81-6. PMID 24214806 DOI: 10.3758/BF03210744 |
0.652 |
|
1994 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Wright JH. The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 20: 317-29. PMID 8189195 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.20.2.317 |
0.542 |
|
1994 |
Folk CL, Annett S. Do locally defined feature discontinuities capture attention? Perception & Psychophysics. 56: 277-87. PMID 7971128 DOI: 10.3758/BF03209762 |
0.598 |
|
1994 |
Iavecchia HP, Folk CL. Shifting visual attention in stereographic displays: a time course analysis. Human Factors. 36: 606-18. PMID 7875690 |
0.489 |
|
1993 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Johnston JC. Contingent Attentional Capture: A Reply to Yantis (1993) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 19: 682-685. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.3.682 |
0.547 |
|
1992 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Johnston JC. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 18: 1030-44. PMID 1431742 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030 |
0.616 |
|
1992 |
McCann RS, Folk CL, Johnston JC. The role of spatial attention in visual word processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 18: 1015-29. PMID 1431741 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.18.4.1015 |
0.555 |
|
1992 |
Folk CL, Hoyer WJ. Aging and shifts of visual spatial attention. Psychology and Aging. 7: 453-65. PMID 1388867 DOI: 10.1037//0882-7974.7.3.453 |
0.523 |
|
1989 |
Folk CL, Egeth H. Does the identification of simple features require serial processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 15: 97-110. PMID 2522536 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.15.1.97 |
0.727 |
|
1988 |
Folk CL, Egeth H, Kwak HW. Subitizing: direct apprehension or serial processing? Perception & Psychophysics. 44: 313-20. PMID 3226878 DOI: 10.3758/BF03210412 |
0.712 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |