Year |
Citation |
Score |
2023 |
Sulikowski D, Favelle S, McKone E, Willis M, Burke D. The composite effect reveals that human (but not other primate) faces are special to humans. Plos One. 18: e0286451. PMID 37252925 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286451 |
0.709 |
|
2020 |
Marshall P, Bartolacci A, Burke D. Human Face Tilt Is a Dynamic Social Signal That Affects Perceptions of Dimorphism, Attractiveness, and Dominance. Evolutionary Psychology : An International Journal of Evolutionary Approaches to Psychology and Behavior. 18: 1474704920910403. PMID 32124644 DOI: 10.1177/1474704920910403 |
0.399 |
|
2017 |
Woo KL, Rieucau G, Burke D. Computer-animated stimuli to measure motion sensitivity: constraints on signal design in the Jacky dragon. Current Zoology. 63: 75-84. PMID 29491965 DOI: 10.1093/cz/zow074 |
0.387 |
|
2016 |
Russell F, Burke D. Conditional same/different concept learning in the short-beaked echidna (Tachyglossus aculeatus). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 105: 133-54. PMID 26781053 DOI: 10.1002/jeab.185 |
0.369 |
|
2015 |
Favelle S, Tobin A, Piepers D, Burke D, Robbins RA. Dynamic composite faces are processed holistically. Vision Research. 112: 26-32. PMID 25982714 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2015.05.002 |
0.734 |
|
2015 |
Favelle S, Tobin A, Piepers D, Burke D, Robbins RA. Dynamic composite faces are processed holistically Vision Research. 112: 26-32. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.05.002 |
0.685 |
|
2013 |
Bennetts RJ, Kim J, Burke D, Brooks KR, Lucey S, Saragih J, Robbins RA. The movement advantage in famous and unfamiliar faces: a comparison of point-light displays and shape-normalised avatar stimuli. Perception. 42: 950-70. PMID 24386715 DOI: 10.1068/P7446 |
0.4 |
|
2013 |
Burke D, Nolan C, Hayward WG, Russell R, Sulikowski D. Is there an own-race preference in attractiveness? Evolutionary Psychology : An International Journal of Evolutionary Approaches to Psychology and Behavior. 11: 855-72. PMID 23948346 DOI: 10.1177/147470491301100410 |
0.307 |
|
2013 |
Burke D, Sulikowski D. The evolution of holistic processing of faces. Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 11. PMID 23382721 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00011 |
0.376 |
|
2012 |
Bennetts R, Burke D, Brooks K, Kim J, Lucey S, Saragih J, Robbins R. Movement helps famous and unfamiliar face matching: Evidence from a sorting task Journal of Vision. 12: 981-981. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.981 |
0.305 |
|
2012 |
Sulikowski D, Burke D. Win shifting in nectarivorous birds: Selective inhibition of the learned win-stay response Animal Behaviour. 83: 519-524. DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.030 |
0.322 |
|
2011 |
Willis ML, Palermo R, Burke D. Judging approachability on the face of it: the influence of face and body expressions on the perception of approachability. Emotion (Washington, D.C.). 11: 514-23. PMID 21668104 DOI: 10.1037/a0022571 |
0.306 |
|
2011 |
Sulikowski D, Burke D. Win-shift and win-stay learning in the rainbow lorikeet (Trichoglossus haematodus). Journal of Comparative Psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983). 125: 143-9. PMID 21604850 DOI: 10.1037/a0023249 |
0.341 |
|
2011 |
Willis ML, Palermo R, Burke D. Social judgments are influenced by both facial expression and direction of eye gaze Social Cognition. 29: 415-429. DOI: 10.1521/soco.2011.29.4.415 |
0.301 |
|
2011 |
Sulikowski D, Burke D. Movement and memory: Different cognitive strategies are used to search for resources with different natural distributions Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 65: 621-631. DOI: 10.1007/s00265-010-1063-4 |
0.303 |
|
2010 |
Burke D, Sulikowski D. A new viewpoint on the evolution of sexually dimorphic human faces. Evolutionary Psychology : An International Journal of Evolutionary Approaches to Psychology and Behavior. 8: 573-85. PMID 22947821 DOI: 10.1177/147470491000800404 |
0.309 |
|
2010 |
Willis ML, Palermo R, Burke D, Atkinson CM, McArthur G. Switching associations between facial identity and emotional expression: a behavioural and ERP study. Neuroimage. 50: 329-39. PMID 19962443 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.071 |
0.344 |
|
2010 |
Taubert J, Burke D, Favelle S, McKone E. Navigating the boundary of face space: What kind of stimulus is a face? Journal of Vision. 7: 889-889. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.889 |
0.703 |
|
2010 |
Sulikowski D, Burke D. When a place is not a place: Encoding of spatial information is dependent on reward type Behaviour. 147: 1461-1479. DOI: 10.1163/000579510X521564 |
0.321 |
|
2009 |
Campbell J, Burke D. Evidence that identity-dependent and identity-independent neural populations are recruited in the perception of five basic emotional facial expressions. Vision Research. 49: 1532-40. PMID 19303422 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.03.009 |
0.36 |
|
2009 |
Woo KL, Burke D, Peters RA. Motion sensitivity of the Jacky dragon, Amphibolurus muricatus: random-dot kinematograms reveal the importance of motion noise for signal detection Animal Behaviour. 77: 307-315. DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.10.011 |
0.31 |
|
2008 |
Tomiczek C, Burke D. Is implicit learning perceptually inflexible? New evidence using a simple cued reaction-time task Learning and Motivation. 39: 95-113. DOI: 10.1016/J.Lmot.2007.07.001 |
0.742 |
|
2007 |
Favelle SK, Burke D. Attention to configural information in change detection for faces. Perception. 36: 1353-67. PMID 18196701 DOI: 10.1068/p5517 |
0.723 |
|
2007 |
Burke D, Taubert J, Higman T. Are face representations viewpoint dependent? A stereo advantage for generalizing across different views of faces. Vision Research. 47: 2164-9. PMID 17572467 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.04.018 |
0.382 |
|
2006 |
Favelle SK, Hayward WG, Burke D, Palmisano S. The configural advantage in object change detection persists across depth rotation. Perception & Psychophysics. 68: 1254-63. PMID 17378412 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193725 |
0.749 |
|
2006 |
Wenderoth P, Burke D. Testing the tilt-constancy theory of visual illusions. Perception. 35: 201-13. PMID 16583766 DOI: 10.1068/p5388 |
0.603 |
|
2006 |
Favelle S, Palmisano S, Burke D, Hayward WG. The role of attention in processing configural and shape information in 3-D novel objects Visual Cognition. 13: 623-642. DOI: 10.1080/13506280544000228 |
0.759 |
|
2005 |
Burke D. Combining disparate views of objects: Viewpoint costs are reduced by stereopsis Visual Cognition. 12: 705-719. DOI: 10.1080/13506280444000463 |
0.336 |
|
2003 |
Palmisano S, Burke D, Allison RS. Coherent perspective jitter induces visual illusions of self-motion. Perception. 32: 97-110. PMID 12613789 DOI: 10.1068/p3468 |
0.571 |
|
2003 |
Keane SK, Hayward WG, Burke D. Detection of three types of changes to novel objects Visual Cognition. 10: 101-127. DOI: 10.1080/713756672 |
0.366 |
|
2003 |
Burke D, Fulham BJ. An evolved spatial memory bias in a nectar-feeding bird? Animal Behaviour. 66: 695-701. DOI: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2246 |
0.338 |
|
2002 |
Burke D, Cieplucha C, Cass J, Russell F, Fry G. Win-shift and win-stay learning in the short-beaked echidna (Tachyglossus aculeatus). Animal Cognition. 5: 79-84. PMID 12150039 DOI: 10.1007/S10071-002-0131-1 |
0.711 |
|
2002 |
Burke D, Hayward WG. Two visual systems but only one theory of perception Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 25: 100. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X02250025 |
0.389 |
|
2001 |
Burke D, Everingham P, Rogers T, Hinton M, Hall-Aspland S. Perceptual grouping in two visually reliant species: Humans (Homo sapiens) and Australian sea lions (Neophoca cinerea) Perception. 30: 1093-1106. PMID 11694085 DOI: 10.1068/p3239 |
0.336 |
|
2000 |
Burke D, Roodenrys S. Implicit Learning in a Simple Cued Reaction-Time Task Learning and Motivation. 31: 364-380. DOI: 10.1006/lmot.2000.1062 |
0.317 |
|
1999 |
Burke D, Alais D, Wenderoth P. Determinants of fusion of dichoptically presented orthogonal gratings. Perception. 28: 73-88. PMID 10627854 DOI: 10.1068/P2694 |
0.671 |
|
1997 |
Alais D, Wenderoth P, Burke D. The size and number of plaid blobs mediate the misperception of type-II plaid direction. Vision Research. 37: 143-50. PMID 9068836 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00320-7 |
0.699 |
|
1996 |
Alais D, Burke D, Wenderoth P. Further evidence for monocular determinants of perceived plaid direction. Vision Research. 36: 1247-53. PMID 8711904 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00194-8 |
0.704 |
|
1994 |
Burke D, Alais D, Wenderoth P. A role for a low level mechanism in determining plaid coherence. Vision Research. 34: 3189-96. PMID 7975350 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90083-3 |
0.688 |
|
1994 |
Alais D, Wenderoth P, Burke D. The contribution of one-dimensional motion mechanisms to the perceived direction of drifting plaids and their after effects. Vision Research. 34: 1823-34. PMID 7941385 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90307-7 |
0.701 |
|
1994 |
Wenderoth P, Alais D, Burke D, van der Zwan R. The role of the blobs in determining the perception of drifting plaids and their motion aftereffects. Perception. 23: 1163-9. PMID 7899031 DOI: 10.1068/P231163 |
0.766 |
|
1993 |
Burke D, Wenderoth P. Determinants of two-dimensional motion aftereffects induced by simultaneously- and alternately-presented plaid components. Vision Research. 33: 351-9. PMID 8447107 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90091-A |
0.624 |
|
1993 |
Burke D, Wenderoth P. The effect of interactions between one-dimensional component gratings on two-dimensional motion perception. Vision Research. 33: 343-50. PMID 8447106 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90090-J |
0.595 |
|
1989 |
Burke D, Wenderoth P. Cyclopean tilt aftereffects can be induced monocularly: is there a purely binocular process? Perception. 18: 471-82. PMID 2813023 DOI: 10.1068/P180471 |
0.62 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |