Year |
Citation |
Score |
2024 |
Van der Biest M, Pedinoff R, Verbruggen F, Brass M, Kuhlen AK. Instructing somebody else to act: motor co-representations in the instructor. Royal Society Open Science. 11: 230839. PMID 38204793 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.230839 |
0.354 |
|
2023 |
Doekemeijer RA, Dewulf A, Verbruggen F, Boehler CN. Proactively Adjusting Stopping: Response Inhibition is Faster when Stopping Occurs Frequently. Journal of Cognition. 6: 22. PMID 37152832 DOI: 10.5334/joc.264 |
0.367 |
|
2022 |
Dorison CA, Lerner JS, Heller BH, Rothman AJ, Kawachi II, Wang K, Rees VW, Gill BP, Gibbs N, Ebersole CR, Vally Z, Tajchman Z, Zsido AN, Zrimsek M, Chen Z, ... ... Verbruggen F, et al. In COVID-19 Health Messaging, Loss Framing Increases Anxiety with Little-to-No Concomitant Benefits: Experimental Evidence from 84 Countries. Affective Science. 1-26. PMID 36185503 DOI: 10.1007/s42761-022-00128-3 |
0.461 |
|
2022 |
Wang K, Goldenberg A, Dorison CA, Miller JK, Uusberg A, Lerner JS, Gross JJ, Agesin BB, Bernardo M, Campos O, Eudave L, Grzech K, Ozery DH, Jackson EA, Garcia EOL, ... ... Verbruggen F, et al. Author Correction: A multi-country test of brief reappraisal interventions on emotions during the COVID-19 pandemic. Nature Human Behaviour. PMID 36002766 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-022-01441-4 |
0.409 |
|
2021 |
Reimer CB, Chen Z, Verbruggen F. Benefits and costs of self-paced preparation of novel task instructions. Royal Society Open Science. 8: 210762. PMID 34754496 DOI: 10.1098/rsos.210762 |
0.35 |
|
2021 |
Wang K, Goldenberg A, Dorison CA, Miller JK, Uusberg A, Lerner JS, Gross JJ, Agesin BB, Bernardo M, Campos O, Eudave L, Grzech K, Ozery DH, Jackson EA, Garcia EOL, ... ... Verbruggen F, et al. A multi-country test of brief reappraisal interventions on emotions during the COVID-19 pandemic. Nature Human Behaviour. PMID 34341554 DOI: 10.1038/s41562-021-01173-x |
0.43 |
|
2020 |
Bundt C, Boehler CN, Verbruggen F, Brass M, Notebaert W. Reward does not modulate corticospinal excitability in anticipation of a Stroop trial. The European Journal of Neuroscience. PMID 33222331 DOI: 10.1111/ejn.15052 |
0.333 |
|
2020 |
Maizey L, Evans CJ, Muhlert N, Verbruggen F, Chambers CD, Allen CPG. Cortical and subcortical functional specificity associated with response inhibition. Neuroimage. 220: 117110. PMID 32619711 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuroimage.2020.117110 |
0.377 |
|
2020 |
Eben C, Chen Z, Vermeylen L, Billieux J, Verbruggen F. A direct and conceptual replication of post-loss speeding when gambling. Royal Society Open Science. 7: 200090. PMID 32537216 DOI: 10.1098/Rsos.200090 |
0.434 |
|
2020 |
Eben C, Billieux J, Verbruggen F. Clarifying the Role of Negative Emotions in the Origin and Control of Impulsive Actions. Psychologica Belgica. 60: 1-17. PMID 31915527 DOI: 10.5334/Pb.502 |
0.304 |
|
2019 |
Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F. On the Assimilation of Instructions: Stimulus-response Associations are Implemented but not Stimulus-task Associations. Journal of Cognition. 2: 20. PMID 31517238 DOI: 10.5334/joc.78 |
0.465 |
|
2019 |
Best M, Verbruggen F. Does Learning Influence the Detection of Signals in a Response-Inhibition Task? Journal of Cognition. 2: 19. PMID 31517237 DOI: 10.5334/joc.73 |
0.422 |
|
2019 |
Longman CS, Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F. How Does the (Re)Presentation of Instructions Influence Their Implementation? Journal of Cognition. 2: 10. PMID 31517230 DOI: 10.5334/joc.63 |
0.321 |
|
2019 |
Best M, McLaren IPL, Verbruggen F. Instructed and Acquired Contingencies in Response-Inhibition Tasks. Journal of Cognition. 2: 4. PMID 31517225 DOI: 10.5334/joc.53 |
0.474 |
|
2019 |
Lea SEG, Chow PKY, Meier C, McLaren IPL, Verbruggen F. Pigeons' performance in a tracking change-signal procedure is consistent with the independent horse-race model. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Learning and Cognition. PMID 31368768 DOI: 10.1037/Xan0000219 |
0.493 |
|
2019 |
Bundt C, Bardi L, Verbruggen F, Boehler CN, Brass M, Notebaert W. Reward anticipation changes corticospinal excitability during task preparation depending on response requirements and time pressure. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 120: 159-168. PMID 31319357 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cortex.2019.05.020 |
0.437 |
|
2019 |
Verbruggen F, Aron AR, Band GP, Beste C, Bissett PG, Brockett AT, Brown JW, Chamberlain SR, Chambers CD, Colonius H, Colzato LS, Corneil BD, Coxon JP, Dupuis A, Eagle DM, et al. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. Elife. 8. PMID 31033438 DOI: 10.7554/Elife.46323 |
0.801 |
|
2019 |
Verbruggen F, Aron AR, Band GP, Beste C, Bissett PG, Brockett AT, Brown JW, Chamberlain SR, Chambers CD, Colonius H, Colzato LS, Corneil BD, Coxon JP, Dupuis A, Eagle DM, et al. Author response: A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task Elife. DOI: 10.7554/Elife.46323.027 |
0.801 |
|
2018 |
Longman CS, Kiesel A, Verbruggen F. Learning in the absence of overt practice: a novel (previously unseen) stimulus can trigger retrieval of an unpracticed response. Psychological Research. PMID 30415378 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-018-1106-4 |
0.389 |
|
2018 |
Verbruggen F, McLaren R, Pereg M, Meiran N. Structure and Implementation of Novel Task Rules: A Cross-Sectional Developmental Study. Psychological Science. 956797618755322. PMID 29746205 DOI: 10.1177/0956797618755322 |
0.441 |
|
2018 |
McLaren IP, McAndrew A, Angerer K, McLaren R, Forrest C, Bowditch WA, Monsell S, Verbruggen F. Author accepted manuscript: Mackintosh Lecture: Association and Cognition: Two Processes, One System. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 1747021818766287. PMID 29516768 DOI: 10.1177/1747021818766287 |
0.333 |
|
2018 |
Allen C, Singh KD, Verbruggen F, Chambers CD. Evidence for parallel activation of the pre-supplementary motor area and inferior frontal cortex during response inhibition: a combined MEG and TMS study. Royal Society Open Science. 5: 171369. PMID 29515852 DOI: 10.1098/Rsos.171369 |
0.316 |
|
2017 |
Meier C, Pant SR, van Horik JO, Laker PR, Langley EJG, Whiteside MA, Verbruggen F, Madden JR. A novel continuous inhibitory-control task: variation in individual performance by young pheasants (Phasianus colchicus). Animal Cognition. PMID 28795236 DOI: 10.1007/S10071-017-1120-8 |
0.446 |
|
2017 |
Verbruggen F, McLaren R. Development of between-trial response strategy adjustments in a continuous action control task: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 162: 39-57. PMID 28578245 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jecp.2017.05.002 |
0.451 |
|
2017 |
Porter L, Bailey-Jones C, Priudokaite G, Allen S, Wood K, Stiles K, Parvin O, Javaid M, Verbruggen F, Lawrence NS. From cookies to carrots; the effect of inhibitory control training on children's snack selections. Appetite. PMID 28479406 DOI: 10.1016/J.Appet.2017.05.010 |
0.312 |
|
2017 |
Longman CS, Milton F, Wills AJ, Verbruggen F. Transfer of learned category-response associations is modulated by instruction. Acta Psychologica. PMID 28454893 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2017.04.004 |
0.323 |
|
2016 |
Verbruggen F. Executive Control of Actions Across Time and Space. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 25: 399-404. PMID 28018053 DOI: 10.1177/0963721416659254 |
0.348 |
|
2016 |
Verbruggen F, McLaren R. Effects of reward and punishment on the interaction between going and stopping in a selective stop-change task. Psychological Research. PMID 27888354 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-016-0827-5 |
0.488 |
|
2016 |
Adams RC, Lawrence NS, Verbruggen F, Chambers CD. Training response inhibition to reduce food consumption: Mechanisms, stimulus specificity and appropriate training protocols. Appetite. PMID 27838443 DOI: 10.1016/J.Appet.2016.11.014 |
0.397 |
|
2016 |
Verbruggen F, Chambers CD, Lawrence NS, McLaren IP. Winning and Losing: Effects on Impulsive Action. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 27808548 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000284 |
0.49 |
|
2016 |
Elchlepp H, Verbruggen F. How to withhold or replace a prepotent response: An analysis of the underlying control processes and their temporal dynamics. Biological Psychology. 123: 250-268. PMID 27756580 DOI: 10.1016/J.Biopsycho.2016.10.005 |
0.547 |
|
2016 |
Leiva A, Andrés P, Servera M, Verbruggen F, Parmentier FB. The Role of Age, Working Memory, and Response Inhibition in Deviance Distraction: A Cross-Sectional Study. Developmental Psychology. PMID 27505694 DOI: 10.1037/Dev0000163 |
0.393 |
|
2016 |
Civile C, Verbruggen F, McLaren R, Zhao D, Ku Y, McLaren IP. Switching off perceptual learning: Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) at Fp3 eliminates perceptual learning in humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Learning and Cognition. 42: 290-296. PMID 27379720 DOI: 10.1037/Xan0000107 |
0.305 |
|
2016 |
Verbruggen F, McAndrew A, Weidemann G, Stevens T, McLaren IP. Limits of Executive Control: Sequential Effects in Predictable Environments. Psychological Science. PMID 27000177 DOI: 10.1177/0956797616631990 |
0.428 |
|
2016 |
Elchlepp H, Lavric A, Chambers CD, Verbruggen F. Proactive inhibitory control: A general biasing account. Cognitive Psychology. 86: 27-61. PMID 26859519 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogpsych.2016.01.004 |
0.575 |
|
2016 |
Noël X, Brevers D, Hanak C, Kornreich C, Verbanck P, Verbruggen F. On the automaticity of response inhibition in individuals with alcoholism. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 51: 84-91. PMID 26800080 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jbtep.2016.01.003 |
0.366 |
|
2015 |
Bowditch WA, Verbruggen F, McLaren IP. Associatively mediated stopping: Training stimulus-specific inhibitory control. Learning & Behavior. PMID 26400499 DOI: 10.3758/S13420-015-0196-8 |
0.459 |
|
2015 |
Best M, Lawrence NS, Logan GD, McLaren IP, Verbruggen F. Should I Stop or Should I Go? The Role of Associations and Expectancies. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 26322688 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000116 |
0.695 |
|
2015 |
Leiva A, Parmentier FB, Elchlepp H, Verbruggen F. Reorienting the mind: The impact of novel sounds on go/no-go performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 41: 1197-202. PMID 26191617 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000111 |
0.459 |
|
2015 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Evidence for capacity sharing when stopping. Cognition. 142: 81-95. PMID 26036922 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2015.05.014 |
0.703 |
|
2015 |
Stevens T, Brevers D, Chambers CD, Lavric A, McLaren IP, Mertens M, Noël X, Verbruggen F. How does response inhibition influence decision making when gambling? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied. 21: 15-36. PMID 25559481 DOI: 10.1037/Xap0000039 |
0.528 |
|
2014 |
Verbruggen F, McLaren IP, Chambers CD. Banishing the Control Homunculi in Studies of Action Control and Behavior Change. Perspectives On Psychological Science : a Journal of the Association For Psychological Science. 9: 497-524. PMID 25419227 DOI: 10.1177/1745691614526414 |
0.398 |
|
2014 |
Verbruggen F, Best M, Bowditch WA, Stevens T, McLaren IP. The inhibitory control reflex. Neuropsychologia. 65: 263-78. PMID 25149820 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2014.08.014 |
0.41 |
|
2014 |
Verbruggen F, Stevens T, Chambers CD. Proactive and reactive stopping when distracted: an attentional account. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 40: 1295-300. PMID 24842070 DOI: 10.1037/A0036542 |
0.494 |
|
2014 |
Logan GD, Van Zandt T, Verbruggen F, Wagenmakers EJ. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: general and special theories of an act of control. Psychological Review. 121: 66-95. PMID 24490789 DOI: 10.1037/A0035230 |
0.657 |
|
2014 |
Lawrence N, Verbruggen F, Parslow D, O'Sullivan J, Javaid M, Morrison S, Adams R, Chambers C. Response inhibition training to reduce overeating: Translational studies from the lab to real life Appetite. 83: 343. DOI: 10.1016/J.Appet.2014.06.035 |
0.304 |
|
2013 |
Verbruggen F, Adams RC, van 't Wout F, Stevens T, McLaren IP, Chambers CD. Are the effects of response inhibition on gambling long-lasting? Plos One. 8: e70155. PMID 23922948 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0070155 |
0.36 |
|
2013 |
Verbruggen F, Chambers CD, Logan GD. Fictitious inhibitory differences: how skewness and slowing distort the estimation of stopping latencies. Psychological Science. 24: 352-62. PMID 23399493 DOI: 10.1177/0956797612457390 |
0.557 |
|
2013 |
Rusconi E, Dervinis M, Verbruggen F, Chambers CD. Critical time course of right frontoparietal involvement in mental number space. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 25: 465-83. PMID 23163421 DOI: 10.1162/Jocn_A_00330 |
0.324 |
|
2013 |
Noël X, Van der Linden M, Brevers D, Campanella S, Verbanck P, Hanak C, Kornreich C, Verbruggen F. Separating intentional inhibition of prepotent responses and resistance to proactive interference in alcohol-dependent individuals. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 128: 200-5. PMID 22980674 DOI: 10.1016/J.Drugalcdep.2012.08.021 |
0.478 |
|
2012 |
Oldenburg JF, Roger C, Assecondi S, Verbruggen F, Fias W. Repetition priming in the stop signal task: the electrophysiology of sequential effects of stopping. Neuropsychologia. 50: 2860-8. PMID 22940427 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2012.08.014 |
0.495 |
|
2012 |
Jahfari S, Verbruggen F, Frank MJ, Waldorp LJ, Colzato L, Ridderinkhof KR, Forstmann BU. How preparation changes the need for top-down control of the basal ganglia when inhibiting premature actions. The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the Society For Neuroscience. 32: 10870-8. PMID 22875921 DOI: 10.1523/Jneurosci.0902-12.2012 |
0.423 |
|
2012 |
Verbruggen F, Adams R, Chambers CD. Proactive motor control reduces monetary risk taking in gambling. Psychological Science. 23: 805-15. PMID 22692336 DOI: 10.1177/0956797611434538 |
0.394 |
|
2012 |
Chiu YC, Aron AR, Verbruggen F. Response suppression by automatic retrieval of stimulus-stop association: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 24: 1908-18. PMID 22624606 DOI: 10.1162/Jocn_A_00247 |
0.458 |
|
2012 |
Vandierendonck A, Demanet J, Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F. A chain-retrieval model for voluntary task switching. Cognitive Psychology. 65: 241-83. PMID 22609806 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogpsych.2012.04.003 |
0.686 |
|
2012 |
Cai W, George JS, Verbruggen F, Chambers CD, Aron AR. The role of the right presupplementary motor area in stopping action: two studies with event-related transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 108: 380-9. PMID 22514296 DOI: 10.1152/Jn.00132.2012 |
0.387 |
|
2012 |
Majid DS, Cai W, George JS, Verbruggen F, Aron AR. Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals dissociable mechanisms for global versus selective corticomotor suppression underlying the stopping of action. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991). 22: 363-71. PMID 21666129 DOI: 10.1093/Cercor/Bhr112 |
0.449 |
|
2011 |
Demanet J, Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F. Valence, arousal, and cognitive control: a voluntary task-switching study. Frontiers in Psychology. 2: 336. PMID 22131982 DOI: 10.3389/Fpsyg.2011.00336 |
0.494 |
|
2011 |
Klemen J, Verbruggen F, Skelton C, Chambers CD. Enhancement of perceptual representations by endogenous attention biases competition in response selection. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 73: 2514-27. PMID 21826553 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-011-0188-5 |
0.438 |
|
2011 |
Lenartowicz A, Verbruggen F, Logan GD, Poldrack RA. Inhibition-related activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus in the absence of inhibitory cues. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 23: 3388-99. PMID 21452946 DOI: 10.1162/Jocn_A_00031 |
0.654 |
|
2011 |
Szmalec A, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A, Kemps E. Control of interference during working memory updating. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 37: 137-51. PMID 20731517 DOI: 10.1037/A0020365 |
0.582 |
|
2010 |
Verbruggen F, Aron AR, Stevens MA, Chambers CD. Theta burst stimulation dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107: 13966-71. PMID 20631303 DOI: 10.1073/Pnas.1001957107 |
0.315 |
|
2010 |
Vandierendonck A, Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F. Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin. 136: 601-26. PMID 20565170 DOI: 10.1037/A0019791 |
0.702 |
|
2010 |
Demanet J, Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Voluntary task switching under load: contribution of top-down and bottom-up factors in goal-directed behavior. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 17: 387-93. PMID 20551363 DOI: 10.3758/Pbr.17.3.387 |
0.71 |
|
2010 |
Woolard AA, Kose S, Woodward ND, Verbruggen F, Logan GD, Heckers S. Intact associative learning in patients with schizophrenia: evidence from a Go/NoGo paradigm. Schizophrenia Research. 122: 131-5. PMID 20226631 DOI: 10.1016/J.Schres.2010.02.1057 |
0.558 |
|
2010 |
Claffey MP, Sheldon S, Stinear CM, Verbruggen F, Aron AR. Having a goal to stop action is associated with advance control of specific motor representations. Neuropsychologia. 48: 541-8. PMID 19879283 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2009.10.015 |
0.395 |
|
2010 |
Jahfari S, Stinear CM, Claffey M, Verbruggen F, Aron AR. Responding with restraint: what are the neurocognitive mechanisms? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 22: 1479-92. PMID 19583473 DOI: 10.1162/Jocn.2009.21307 |
0.427 |
|
2009 |
Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F. Increasing the difficulty of response selection does not increase the switch cost. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expã©Rimentale. 63: 323-7. PMID 20025391 DOI: 10.1037/A0016725 |
0.498 |
|
2009 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Automaticity of cognitive control: goal priming in response-inhibition paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 35: 1381-8. PMID 19686032 DOI: 10.1037/A0016645 |
0.725 |
|
2009 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 35: 835-54. PMID 19485695 DOI: 10.1037/A0012726 |
0.69 |
|
2009 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 33: 647-61. PMID 18822313 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neubiorev.2008.08.014 |
0.591 |
|
2009 |
Szmalec A, Demanet J, Vandierendonck A, Verbruggen F. Investigating the role of conflict resolution in memory updating by means of the one-back choice RT task. Psychological Research. 73: 390-406. PMID 18437414 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-008-0149-3 |
0.717 |
|
2008 |
Aron AR, Verbruggen F. Stop the presses: dissociating a selective from a global mechanism for stopping. Psychological Science. 19: 1146-53. PMID 19076487 DOI: 10.1111/J.1467-9280.2008.02216.X |
0.377 |
|
2008 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 137: 649-72. PMID 18999358 DOI: 10.1037/A0013170 |
0.624 |
|
2008 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. After-effects of goal shifting and response inhibition: a comparison of the stop-change and dual-task paradigms. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 61: 1151-9. PMID 18938760 DOI: 10.1080/17470210801994971 |
0.713 |
|
2008 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Long-term aftereffects of response inhibition: memory retrieval, task goals, and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 34: 1229-35. PMID 18823207 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1229 |
0.607 |
|
2008 |
Verbruggen F, Schneider DW, Logan GD. How to stop and change a response: the role of goal activation in multitasking. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 34: 1212-28. PMID 18823206 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1212 |
0.792 |
|
2008 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 12: 418-24. PMID 18799345 DOI: 10.1016/J.Tics.2008.07.005 |
0.64 |
|
2008 |
Schneider DW, Verbruggen F. Inhibition of irrelevant category-response mappings. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 61: 1629-40. PMID 18609408 DOI: 10.1080/17470210802138511 |
0.703 |
|
2008 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD, Stevens MA. STOP-IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods. 40: 479-83. PMID 18522058 DOI: 10.3758/Brm.40.2.479 |
0.624 |
|
2008 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Short-term aftereffects of response inhibition: repetition priming or between-trial control adjustments? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 34: 413-26. PMID 18377179 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.413 |
0.724 |
|
2008 |
Szmalec A, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A, De Baene W, Verguts T, Notebaert W. Stimulus ambiguity elicits response conflict. Neuroscience Letters. 435: 158-62. PMID 18329807 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neulet.2008.02.023 |
0.726 |
|
2007 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A, Demanet J. Short cue presentations encourage advance task preparation: a recipe to diminish the residual switch cost. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 33: 342-56. PMID 17352616 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.342 |
0.671 |
|
2007 |
Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A, Fias W, Gevers W. Task switching and across-trial distance priming are independent European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 19: 1-16. DOI: 10.1080/09541440500492033 |
0.68 |
|
2007 |
Verbruggen F, De Houwer J. Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm Cognition and Emotion. 21: 391-403. DOI: 10.1080/02699930600625081 |
0.414 |
|
2006 |
Stevens M, Lammertyn J, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A. Tscope: A C library for programming cognitive experiments on the MS windows platform. Behavior Research Methods. 38: 280-6. PMID 16956104 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03192779 |
0.539 |
|
2006 |
Verbruggen F, Notebaert W, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Stimulus- and response-conflict-induced cognitive control in the flanker task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 13: 328-33. PMID 16893003 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193852 |
0.693 |
|
2006 |
Notebaert W, Gevers W, Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B. Top-down and bottom-up sequential modulations of congruency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 13: 112-7. PMID 16724777 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193821 |
0.468 |
|
2006 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Selective stopping in task switching: The role of response selection and response execution. Experimental Psychology. 53: 48-57. PMID 16610272 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.48 |
0.673 |
|
2006 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. The effect of interference in the early processing stages on response inhibition in the stop signal task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 59: 190-203. PMID 16556567 DOI: 10.1080/17470210500151386 |
0.707 |
|
2005 |
Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A, Muyllaert I, Verbruggen F, Vanneste W. The phonological loop in task alternation and task repetition. Memory (Hove, England). 13: 550-60. PMID 16020382 DOI: 10.1080/09658210444000250 |
0.699 |
|
2005 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Notebaert W, Vandierendonck A. Effects of stimulus-stimulus compatibility and stimulus-response compatibility on response inhibition. Acta Psychologica. 120: 307-26. PMID 15993830 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2005.05.003 |
0.732 |
|
2005 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Szmalec A, Vandierendonck A. Inhibiting responses when switching: Does it matter? Experimental Psychology. 52: 125-30. PMID 15850159 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169.52.2.125 |
0.714 |
|
2005 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. On the difference between response inhibition and negative priming: evidence from simple and selective stopping. Psychological Research. 69: 262-71. PMID 15750869 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-004-0177-6 |
0.689 |
|
2005 |
Notebaert W, Verbruggen F, Soetens E. A sequential analysis of relevant and irrelevant information in the Stroop task European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 17: 642-658. DOI: 10.1080/09541440540000095 |
0.445 |
|
2004 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. The interaction between stop signal inhibition and distractor interference in the flanker and Stroop task. Acta Psychologica. 116: 21-37. PMID 15111228 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2003.12.011 |
0.739 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |