Ross Henry Day - Publications

Affiliations: 
1955-1965 University of Sydney, Camperdown, New South Wales, Australia 
 1965-1992 Monash University, Caulfield East, Victoria, Australia 
 1992-2018 La Trobe University, Flora Hill, Victoria, Australia 
Area:
vision
Website:
https://www.science.org.au/learning/general-audience/history/interviews-australian-scientists/professor-ross-day-psychology

41 high-probability publications. We are testing a new system for linking publications to authors. You can help! If you notice any inaccuracies, please sign in and mark papers as correct or incorrect matches. If you identify any major omissions or other inaccuracies in the publication list, please let us know.

Year Citation  Score
2012 Stuart GW, Lambeth SE, Day RH, Gould IC, Castles AE. The role of the magnocellular visual pathway in the attentional blink. Brain and Cognition. 78: 99-104. PMID 22218296 DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.12.002  0.616
2003 Best CJ, Day RH, Crassini B. The influence of object-image velocity change on perceived heading in minimal environments. Perception & Psychophysics. 65: 1273-84. PMID 14710961 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03194851  0.655
2002 Best CJ, Crassini B, Day RH. The roles of static depth information and object-image relative motion in perception of heading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 28: 884-901. PMID 12190256 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.4.884  0.64
1999 Crassini B, Broerse J, Day RH, Best CJ, Sparrow WA. What is the point of attempting to make a case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 22: 372-373. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X99292023  0.655
1993 Day RH, Stecher EJ, Parker AL. The effect of edge orientation and movement direction on the apparent misalignment of collinear bars. Perception & Psychophysics. 53: 642-7. PMID 8332430 DOI: 10.3758/BF03211740  0.389
1993 Day RH, Stecher EJ, Parker AL. The Poggendorff illusion and apparent interparallel extents. Perception. 21: 599-610. PMID 1488263 DOI: 10.1068/p210599  0.31
1992 Webster WR, Day RH, Gillies O, Crassini B. Spatial-frequency-contingent color aftereffects: adaptation with two-dimensional stimulus patterns. Perception & Psychophysics. 51: 66-78. PMID 1549426 DOI: 10.3758/BF03205075  0.715
1992 Day RH, Webster WR, Gillies O, Crassini B. Spatial-frequency-contingent color aftereffects: adaptation with one-dimensional stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics. 51: 57-65. PMID 1549425 DOI: 10.3758/BF03205074  0.713
1992 Webster WR, Day RH, Cassell JF. Two movement aftereffects: evidence for luminance- and color-movement pathways. Vision Research. 32: 2187-9. PMID 1304095 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90079-X  0.646
1990 Gordon IE, Day RH, Stecher EJ. Perceived causality occurs with stroboscopic movement of one or both stimulus elements. Perception. 19: 17-20. PMID 2336330 DOI: 10.1068/p190017  0.382
1989 Day RH, Webster WR. Negative afterimages and the McCollough effect. Perception & Psychophysics. 46: 419-24. PMID 2813026 DOI: 10.3758/BF03210856  0.58
1988 Webster WR, Day RH, Willenberg K. Orientation-contingent color aftereffects are determined by real color, not induced color. Perception & Psychophysics. 44: 43-9. PMID 3405728 DOI: 10.3758/BF03207474  0.575
1988 Day RH, Wade NJ. Binocular interaction in induced rotary motion Australian Journal of Psychology. 40: 159-164. DOI: 10.1080/00049538808259079  0.465
1987 Swanston MT, Wade NJ, Day RH. The representation of uniform motion in vision. Perception. 16: 143-59. PMID 3684477 DOI: 10.1068/P160143  0.513
1986 Glass C, Bradshaw JL, Day RH, Umiltà C. Familiarity, spatial frequency and task determinants in processing laterally presented representations of faces. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 21: 513-31. PMID 4092482 DOI: 10.1016/S0010-9452(58)80002-7  0.329
1984 Day RH, Kasperczyk RT. Illusory contours in line patterns with apparent depth due to either perspective or overlay. Perception. 12: 485-90. PMID 6672745 DOI: 10.1068/p120485  0.335
1981 Day RH, Knuth H. The contributions of F C Müller-Lyer. Perception. 10: 126-46. PMID 7024901 DOI: 10.1068/p100126  0.329
1981 Day RH, McKenzie BE. Infant Perception of the Invariant Size of Approaching and Receding Objects Developmental Psychology. 17: 670-677. DOI: 10.1037//0012-1649.17.5.670  0.385
1980 Riggs LA, Day RH. Visual aftereffects derived from inspection of orthogonally moving patterns. Science (New York, N.Y.). 208: 416-8. PMID 7367869  0.573
1980 McKenzie BE, Tootell HE, Day RH. Development of visual size constancy during the 1st year of human infancy. Developmental Psychology. 16: 163-174. DOI: 10.1037//0012-1649.16.3.163  0.361
1979 Day RH, Wade NJ. Absence of color selectivity in the visual movement aftereffect. Perception & Psychophysics. 25: 111-4. PMID 432094 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03198795  0.591
1979 Schwartz M, Day RH, Cohen LB. Visual Shape Perception in Early Infancy Monographs of the Society For Research in Child Development. 44: 1. DOI: 10.2307/1165963  0.338
1978 Wade NJ, Day RH. On the colors seen in achromatic patterns Perception & Psychophysics. 23: 261-264. DOI: 10.3758/Bf03204136  0.462
1978 Day RH. Induced visual movement as nonveridical resolution of displacement ambiguity Perception & Psychophysics. 23: 205-209. DOI: 10.3758/BF03204126  0.386
1977 Day RH, Dickinson RG. Absence of color-selectivity in Duncker-type induced visual movement Perception & Psychophysics. 22: 313-320. DOI: 10.3758/BF03199696  0.372
1975 Strelow ER, Day RH. Visual movement aftereffect: evidence for independent adaptation to moving target and stationary surround. Vision Research. 15: 117-21. PMID 1129958  0.761
1973 Day RH, McKenzie BE. Perceptual shape constancy in early infancy. Perception. 2: 315-320. PMID 4794127 DOI: 10.1068/p020315  0.365
1973 Wade NJ, Day RH. A correction to “The retinal reference of the tilt aftereffect” by Coltheart and Cooper Perception & Psychophysics. 13: 319-320. DOI: 10.3758/Bf03214146  0.449
1972 Day RH. Visual spatial illusions: a general explanation. Science (New York, N.Y.). 175: 1335-40. PMID 5059563 DOI: 10.1126/science.175.4028.1335  0.387
1971 Day RH, Strelow E. Reduction or disappearance of visual after effect of movement in the absence of patterned surround. Nature. 230: 55-6. PMID 4930968  0.765
1971 Strelow ER, Day RH. Aftereffect of visual movement: Storage in the absence of a patterned surround Perception & Psychophysics. 9: 485-486. DOI: 10.3758/BF03208960  0.764
1969 Day RH, Wade NJ. The reference for visual normalization. The American Journal of Psychology. 82: 191-7. PMID 5811181  0.551
1969 Day RH, Wade NJ. Mechanisms involved in visual orientation constancy. Psychological Bulletin. 71: 33-42. PMID 5312491  0.551
1969 Singer G, Day RH. Visual capture of haptically judged depth Perception & Psychophysics. 5: 315-316. DOI: 10.3758/BF03209572  0.352
1968 Day RH, Wade NJ. Involvement of neck proprioceptive system in visual after-effect from prolonged head tilt. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 20: 290-3. PMID 5683768 DOI: 10.1080/14640746808400163  0.553
1968 Wade NJ, Day RH. Development and dissipation of a visual spatial aftereffect from prolonged head tilt. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 76: 439-43. PMID 5642156  0.575
1968 Wade NJ, Day RH. Apparent head position as a basis for a visual aftereffect of prolonged head tilt Perception & Psychophysics. 3: 324-326. DOI: 10.3758/BF03212477  0.537
1967 Wade NJ, Day RH. Tilt and centrifugation in changing the direction of body-force. The American Journal of Psychology. 80: 637-9. PMID 6081403  0.392
1966 Day RH, Wade NJ. Visual spatial aftereffect from prolonged head-tilt. Science (New York, N.Y.). 154: 1201-2. PMID 5921384  0.553
1966 Landauer AA, Singer G, Day RH. Correlation between visual and kinesthetic spatial aftereffects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 72: 892-894. DOI: 10.1037/h0023861  0.337
1964 Dunlop CW, Mclachlan EM, Webster WR, Day RH. Auditory habituation in cats as a function of stimulus intensity [22] Nature. 203: 874-875. PMID 14204079 DOI: 10.1038/203874a0  0.516
Show low-probability matches.