Year |
Citation |
Score |
2024 |
Grubert A, Wang Z, Williams E, Jimenez M, Remington R, Eimer M. The capacity limitations of multiple-template visual search during task preparation and target selection. Psychophysiology. e14720. PMID 39491049 DOI: 10.1111/psyp.14720 |
0.405 |
|
2022 |
Sisk CA, Toh YN, Jun J, Remington RW, Lee VG. Impact of active and latent concerns about COVID-19 on attention. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications. 7: 48. PMID 35657440 DOI: 10.1186/s41235-022-00401-w |
0.307 |
|
2021 |
Luck SJ, Gaspelin N, Folk CL, Remington RW, Theeuwes J. Progress Toward Resolving the Attentional Capture Debate. Visual Cognition. 29: 1-21. PMID 33574729 DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2020.1848949 |
0.327 |
|
2020 |
Harris AM, Remington RW. Late guidance resolves the search slope paradox in contextual cueing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. PMID 32779117 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-020-01788-7 |
0.651 |
|
2020 |
Harris AM, Jacoby O, Remington RW, Becker SI, Mattingley JB. Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for a dissociation between working memory capacity and feature-based attention. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 129: 158-174. PMID 32473402 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cortex.2020.04.009 |
0.603 |
|
2020 |
Hebert CR, Sha LZ, Remington RW, Jiang YV. Redundancy gain in visual search of simulated X-ray images. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 31907837 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-019-01934-X |
0.311 |
|
2020 |
Harris AM, Jacoby O, Remington RW, Becker SI, Mattingley JB. Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for a dissociation between working memory capacity and feature-based attention Cortex. 129: 158-174. DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.04.009 |
0.511 |
|
2019 |
Sisk CA, Remington RW, Jiang YV. Mechanisms of contextual cueing: A tutorial review. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 31410759 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-019-01832-2 |
0.503 |
|
2019 |
Sisk CA, Remington RW, Jiang YV. A spatial bias toward highly rewarded locations is associated with awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. PMID 31343251 DOI: 10.1037/Xlm0000749 |
0.414 |
|
2019 |
Addleman DA, Schmidt AL, Remington RW, Jiang YV. Implicit location probability learning does not induce baseline shifts of visuospatial attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. PMID 30887446 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-019-01588-8 |
0.489 |
|
2019 |
Jun J, Tan DH, Remington RW, Jiang YV. Memory across a short-delay: Systematic biases in memory for faces. Acta Psychologica. 195: 39-49. PMID 30878000 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2019.02.007 |
0.305 |
|
2019 |
Jun J, Remington RW, Koutstaal W, Jiang YV. Characteristics of sustaining attention in a gradual-onset continuous performance task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 30730177 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000604 |
0.437 |
|
2019 |
Harris AM, Jacoby O, Remington RW, Travis SL, Mattingley JB. Taking a closer look at visual search: Just how feature-agnostic is singleton detection mode? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 30603988 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-018-01642-Y |
0.668 |
|
2018 |
Sha LZ, Remington RW, Jiang YV. Statistical learning of anomalous regions in complex faux X-ray images does not transfer between detection and discrimination. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications. 3: 48. PMID 30547282 DOI: 10.1186/S41235-018-0144-1 |
0.414 |
|
2018 |
Strickland L, Loft S, Remington RW, Heathcote A. Racing to remember: A theory of decision control in event-based prospective memory. Psychological Review. PMID 30080068 DOI: 10.1037/Rev0000113 |
0.319 |
|
2018 |
Li S, Remington R, Jiang Y. Learning-induced changes in attentional priority map are task-specific Journal of Vision. 18: 1182. DOI: 10.1167/18.10.1182 |
0.325 |
|
2017 |
Addleman DA, Tao J, Remington RW, Jiang YV. Explicit Goal-Driven Attention, Unlike Implicitly Learned Attention, Spreads to Secondary Tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 28795835 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000457 |
0.447 |
|
2017 |
Salovich NA, Remington RW, Jiang YV. Acquisition of habitual visual attention and transfer to related tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. PMID 28698989 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-017-1341-5 |
0.388 |
|
2017 |
Sha LZ, Remington RW, Jiang YV. Short-term and long-term attentional biases to frequently encountered target features. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 28439792 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-017-1317-6 |
0.458 |
|
2017 |
Strickland L, Heathcote A, Remington RW, Loft S. Accumulating Evidence About What Prospective Memory Costs Actually Reveal. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. PMID 28383957 DOI: 10.1037/Xlm0000400 |
0.334 |
|
2017 |
Harris AM, Remington RW. Contextual Cueing Improves Attentional Guidance, Even When Guidance Is Supposedly Optimal. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 28230395 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000394 |
0.658 |
|
2017 |
Sha LZ, Remington RW, Jiang YV. Rapid and selective updating of the target template in visual search. Journal of Vision. 17: 36. PMID 28129420 DOI: 10.1167/17.1.36 |
0.448 |
|
2017 |
Colman HA, Remington RW, Kritikos A. Handedness and Graspability Modify Shifts of Visuospatial Attention to Near-Hand Objects. Plos One. 12: e0170542. PMID 28125635 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0170542 |
0.416 |
|
2017 |
Addleman D, Tao J, Remington R, Jiang Y. Explicit goal-driven attention, unlike implicitly learned attention, slips to secondary tasks Journal of Vision. 17: 1331-1331. DOI: 10.1167/17.10.1331 |
0.401 |
|
2016 |
Colman H, Remington R, Kritikos A. Grasping remaps the distribution of visuospatial attention and enhances competing action activation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 70: 1892-1908. PMID 27434754 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1214974 |
0.36 |
|
2016 |
Vromen JM, Lipp OV, Remington RW, Becker SI. Threat captures attention, but not automatically: Top-down goals modulate attentional orienting to threat distractors. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 27234013 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-016-1142-3 |
0.515 |
|
2016 |
Jiang YV, Remington RW, Asaad A, Lee HJ, Mikkalson TC. Remembering Faces and Scenes: The Mixed-Category Advantage in Visual Working Memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 27123683 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000228 |
0.308 |
|
2016 |
Retell JD, Becker SI, Remington RW. Previously seen and expected stimuli elicit surprise in the context of visual search. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 26742498 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-015-1052-9 |
0.469 |
|
2015 |
Sha L, Remington R, Jiang Y. Explicit awareness mediates reward-based prioritization of spatial attention. Journal of Vision. 15: 1348. PMID 26327036 DOI: 10.1167/15.12.1348 |
0.408 |
|
2015 |
Retell JD, Becker SI, Remington RW. An effective attentional set for a specific colour does not prevent capture by infrequently presented motion distractors. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 1-26. PMID 26299891 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1080738 |
0.514 |
|
2015 |
Jiang YV, Lee HJ, Asaad A, Remington R. Similarity effects in visual working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. PMID 26202703 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-015-0905-5 |
0.341 |
|
2015 |
Jiang YV, Sha LZ, Remington RW. Modulation of spatial attention by goals, statistical learning, and monetary reward. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 26105657 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-015-0952-Z |
0.435 |
|
2015 |
Folk CL, Remington RW. Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 41: 1153-65. PMID 26030438 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000084 |
0.49 |
|
2015 |
Harris AM, Becker SI, Remington RW. Capture by colour: Evidence for dimension-specific singleton capture. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 26018643 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-015-0927-0 |
0.676 |
|
2015 |
Heathcote A, Loft S, Remington RW. Slow down and remember to remember! A delay theory of prospective memory costs. Psychological Review. 122: 376-410. PMID 25844878 DOI: 10.1037/A0038952 |
0.338 |
|
2014 |
Vromen JM, Lipp OV, Remington RW. The spider does not always win the fight for attention: Disengagement from threat is modulated by goal set. Cognition & Emotion. 1-12. PMID 25329044 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2014.969198 |
0.504 |
|
2014 |
Venini D, Remington RW, Horstmann G, Becker SI. Centre-of-Gravity Fixations in Visual Search: When Looking at Nothing Helps to Find Something. Journal of Ophthalmology. 2014: 237812. PMID 25002972 DOI: 10.1155/2014/237812 |
0.446 |
|
2014 |
Wu SC, Remington RW, Folk CL. Onsets do not override top-down goals, but they are responded to more quickly. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 76: 649-54. PMID 24596080 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-014-0637-Z |
0.699 |
|
2014 |
Harrison WJ, Remington RW, Mattingley JB. Visual crowding is anisotropic along the horizontal meridian during smooth pursuit. Journal of Vision. 14. PMID 24453347 DOI: 10.1167/14.1.21 |
0.365 |
|
2013 |
Loft S, Smith RE, Remington RW. Minimizing the disruptive effects of prospective memory in simulated air traffic control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied. 19: 254-65. PMID 24059825 DOI: 10.1037/A0034141 |
0.309 |
|
2013 |
Finlayson NJ, Remington RW, Retell JD, Grove PM. Segmentation by depth does not always facilitate visual search. Journal of Vision. 13. PMID 23847304 DOI: 10.1167/13.8.11 |
0.771 |
|
2013 |
Harris AM, Remington RW, Becker SI. Feature specificity in attentional capture by size and color. Journal of Vision. 13. PMID 23650630 DOI: 10.1167/13.3.12 |
0.659 |
|
2013 |
Harrison WJ, Retell JD, Remington RW, Mattingley JB. Visual crowding at a distance during predictive remapping. Current Biology : Cb. 23: 793-8. PMID 23562269 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cub.2013.03.050 |
0.386 |
|
2013 |
Becker SI, Folk CL, Remington RW. Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science. 24: 634-47. PMID 23558547 DOI: 10.1177/0956797612458528 |
0.486 |
|
2013 |
Irons JL, Remington RW. Can attentional control settings be maintained for two color-location conjunctions? Evidence from an RSVP task. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 75: 862-75. PMID 23444082 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-013-0439-8 |
0.521 |
|
2013 |
Harrison WJ, Mattingley JB, Remington RW. Eye movement targets are released from visual crowding. The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the Society For Neuroscience. 33: 2927-33. PMID 23407951 DOI: 10.1523/Jneurosci.4172-12.2013 |
0.368 |
|
2013 |
Loft S, Remington RW. Wait a second: Brief delays in responding reduce focality effects in event-based prospective memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 66: 1432-47. PMID 23281819 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2012.750677 |
0.349 |
|
2013 |
Retell J, Becker S, Remington R. The contributions of expectancy and prior exposure to the surprise response in visual search. Journal of Vision. 13: 165-165. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.165 |
0.413 |
|
2013 |
Finlayson N, Remington R, Retell J, Grove P. The effects of grouping search elements by depth on target selection Journal of Vision. 13: 1242-1242. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.1242 |
0.746 |
|
2012 |
Harrison WJ, Mattingley JB, Remington RW. Pre-saccadic shifts of visual attention. Plos One. 7: e45670. PMID 23029175 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0045670 |
0.469 |
|
2012 |
Finlayson NJ, Remington RW, Grove PM. The role of presentation method and depth singletons in visual search for objects moving in depth. Journal of Vision. 12: 13. PMID 22923724 DOI: 10.1167/12.8.13 |
0.761 |
|
2012 |
Irons JL, Folk CL, Remington RW. All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 38: 758-75. PMID 22201470 DOI: 10.1037/A0026578 |
0.491 |
|
2012 |
Harrison W, Retell J, Remington R, Mattingley J. Predictive remapping preserves elementary visual features across saccades Journal of Vision. 12: 444-444. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.444 |
0.322 |
|
2012 |
Retell J, Kunde A, Lein M, Remington R. The role of reward in driving attention shifts. Journal of Vision. 12: 370-370. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.370 |
0.366 |
|
2012 |
Finlayson N, Remington R, Grove P. The role of presentation and depth singletons in the prioritization of approaching but not receding motion in depth Journal of Vision. 12: 238-238. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.238 |
0.722 |
|
2011 |
Loft S, Finnerty D, Remington RW. Using spatial context to support prospective memory in simulated air traffic control. Human Factors. 53: 662-71. PMID 22235528 DOI: 10.1177/0018720811421783 |
0.313 |
|
2011 |
Remington RW, Wu SC, Pashler H. What determines saccade timing in sequences of coordinated eye and hand movements? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 18: 538-43. PMID 21380589 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-011-0066-0 |
0.611 |
|
2011 |
Harrison W, Remington R, Mattingley J. Is there predictive remapping of visual attention across eye movements? Journal of Vision. 11: 242-242. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.242 |
0.332 |
|
2011 |
Irons JL, Remington RW, McLean JP. Not so fast: Rethinking the effects of action video games on attentional capacity Australian Journal of Psychology. 63: 224-231. DOI: 10.1111/J.1742-9536.2011.00001.X |
0.394 |
|
2011 |
Loft S, Pearcy B, Remington RW. Varying the complexity of the prospective memory decision process in an air traffic control simulation Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology. 219: 77-84. DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/A000051 |
0.332 |
|
2010 |
Becker SI, Folk CL, Remington RW. The role of relational information in contingent capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 36: 1460-76. PMID 20919781 DOI: 10.1037/A0020370 |
0.513 |
|
2010 |
Loft S, Remington RW. Prospective memory and task interference in a continuous monitoring dynamic display task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied. 16: 145-57. PMID 20565199 DOI: 10.1037/A0018900 |
0.371 |
|
2010 |
Folk CL, Remington R. A critical evaluation of the disengagement hypothesis. Acta Psychologica. 135: 103-5; discussion 13. PMID 20510849 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2010.04.012 |
0.502 |
|
2010 |
Becker SI, Folk CL, Remington RW. The role of target-distractor relationships in the guidance of attention Perception. 39: 118-118. DOI: 10.1068/V100035 |
0.445 |
|
2009 |
Ruthruff E, Johnston JC, Remington RW. How strategic is the central bottleneck: can it be overcome by trying harder? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 35: 1368-84. PMID 19803643 DOI: 10.1037/A0015784 |
0.329 |
|
2009 |
Lachter J, Remington RW, Ruthruff E. Space, object, and task selection. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 71: 995-1014. PMID 19525533 DOI: 10.3758/App.71.5.995 |
0.481 |
|
2009 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Wu SC. Additivity of abrupt onset effects supports nonspatial distraction, not the capture of spatial attention. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 71: 308-13. PMID 19304620 DOI: 10.3758/App.71.2.308 |
0.656 |
|
2008 |
Lien MC, Ruthruff E, Goodin Z, Remington RW. Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: converging evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 34: 509-30. PMID 18505320 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.509 |
0.511 |
|
2008 |
Loft S, Kearney R, Remington R. Is task interference in event-based prospective memory dependent on cue presentation? Memory and Cognition. 36: 139-148. PMID 18323070 DOI: 10.3758/Mc.36.1.139 |
0.399 |
|
2008 |
Folk CL, Remington RW. Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings Visual Cognition. 16: 215-231. DOI: 10.1080/13506280701458804 |
0.497 |
|
2006 |
Lien MC, Allen PA, Ruthruff E, Grabbe J, McCann RS, Remington RW. Visual word recognition without central attention: evidence for greater automaticity with advancing age. Psychology and Aging. 21: 431-47. PMID 16953708 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.431 |
0.364 |
|
2006 |
Ruthruff E, Van Selst M, Johnston JC, Remington R. How does practice reduce dual-task interference: integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? Psychological Research. 70: 125-42. PMID 16703392 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-004-0192-7 |
0.333 |
|
2006 |
Hazeltine E, Ruthruff E, Remington RW. The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology. 52: 291-345. PMID 16581054 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogpsych.2005.11.001 |
0.362 |
|
2006 |
Ruthruff E, Hazeltine E, Remington RW. What causes residual dual-task interference after practice? Psychological Research. 70: 494-503. PMID 16184395 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-005-0012-8 |
0.331 |
|
2006 |
Folk CL, Remington R. Top-down modulation of preattentive processing: Testing the recovery account of contingent capture Visual Cognition. 14: 445-465. DOI: 10.1080/13506280500193545 |
0.451 |
|
2005 |
Lien MC, Ruthruff E, Remington RW, Johnston JC. On the limits of advance preparation for a task switch: do people prepare all the task some of the time or some of the task all the time? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 31: 299-315. PMID 15826232 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.299 |
0.311 |
|
2003 |
Wu SC, Remington RW. Characteristics of covert and overt visual orienting: Evidence from attentional and oculomotor capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 29: 1050-67. PMID 14585022 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1050 |
0.696 |
|
2003 |
Ruthruff E, Johnston JC, Van Selst M, Whitsell S, Remington R. Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 29: 280-9. PMID 12760615 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.280 |
0.323 |
|
2003 |
Lien M, Remington RW. The limits of attention: temporal constraints in human information processing. Kimron Shapiro (ed.). Oxford University Press, New York, 2001. No. of pages 264. ISBN 0-19-850516-7. Price: $95.00 (hardback) Applied Cognitive Psychology. 17: 495-496. DOI: 10.1002/Acp.884 |
0.301 |
|
2001 |
Ruthruff E, Remington RW, Johnston JC. Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 27: 1404-19. PMID 11766933 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.6.1404 |
0.325 |
|
2001 |
Remington RW, Folk CL. A dissociation between attention and selection. Psychological Science. 12: 511-5. PMID 11760140 DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00394 |
0.46 |
|
2001 |
Stone M, Dismukes K, Remington R. Prospective memory in dynamic environments: Effects of load, delay, and phonological rehearsal Memory. 9: 165-176. PMID 11469311 DOI: 10.1080/09658210143000100 |
0.345 |
|
2001 |
Remington RW, Folk CL, McLean JP. Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention? Perception & Psychophysics. 63: 298-307. PMID 11281104 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03194470 |
0.419 |
|
2000 |
Remington RW, Johnston JC, Ruthruff E, Gold M, Romera M. Visual search in complex displays: factors affecting conflict detection by air traffic controllers. Human Factors. 42: 349-66. PMID 11132797 DOI: 10.1518/001872000779698105 |
0.313 |
|
2000 |
McCann RS, Remington RW, Van Selst M. A dual-task investigation of automaticity in visual word processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 26: 1352-70. PMID 10946719 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.26.4.1352 |
0.35 |
|
1999 |
Folk CL, Remington R. Can new objects override attentional control settings? Perception & Psychophysics. 61: 727-39. PMID 10370339 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03205541 |
0.485 |
|
1998 |
Folk CL, Remington R. Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 24: 847-58. PMID 9627420 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.3.847 |
0.502 |
|
1996 |
Hanna A, Remington R. The representation of color and form in long-term memory Memory and Cognition. 24: 322-330. PMID 8718766 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03213296 |
0.335 |
|
1995 |
Johnston JC, McCann RS, Remington RW. Chronometric evidence for two types of attention Psychological Science. 6: 365-369. DOI: 10.1111/J.1467-9280.1995.Tb00527.X |
0.383 |
|
1994 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Wright JH. The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 20: 317-29. PMID 8189195 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.20.2.317 |
0.483 |
|
1993 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Johnston JC. Contingent Attentional Capture: A Reply to Yantis (1993) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 19: 682-685. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.3.682 |
0.447 |
|
1992 |
Remington RW, Johnston JC, Yantis S. Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets. Perception & Psychophysics. 51: 279-90. PMID 1561053 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03212254 |
0.474 |
|
1992 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Johnston JC. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 18: 1030-44. PMID 1431742 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030 |
0.503 |
|
1986 |
Remington R, Williams D. ON THE SELECTION AND EVALUATION OF VISUAL DISPLAY SYMBOLOGY: FACTORS INFLUENCING SEARCH AND IDENTIFICATION TIMES Human Factors. 28: 407-420. PMID 3793109 DOI: 10.1177/001872088602800404 |
0.425 |
|
1984 |
Remington R, Pierce L. Moving attention: Evidence for time-invariant shifts of visual selective attention Perception & Psychophysics. 35: 393-399. PMID 6739275 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03206344 |
0.479 |
|
1980 |
Remington RW. Attention and saccadic eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 6: 726-44. PMID 6449540 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.6.4.726 |
0.375 |
|
1979 |
Shulman GL, Remington RW, McLean JP. Moving attention through visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 5: 522-6. PMID 528957 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.5.3.522 |
0.491 |
|
1977 |
Remington R. Processing of phonemes in speech: A spees–accuracy study Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 62: 1279-1290. PMID 915122 DOI: 10.1121/1.381653 |
0.334 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |