Year |
Citation |
Score |
2020 |
Remington RW, Vromen JMG, Becker SI, Baumann O, Mattingley JB. The Role of Frontoparietal Cortex across the Functional Stages of Visual Search. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1-14. PMID 32985948 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01632 |
0.8 |
|
2020 |
Harris AM, Remington RW. Late guidance resolves the search slope paradox in contextual cueing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. PMID 32779117 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-020-01788-7 |
0.96 |
|
2020 |
Harris AM, Jacoby O, Remington RW, Becker SI, Mattingley JB. Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for a dissociation between working memory capacity and feature-based attention. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 129: 158-174. PMID 32473402 DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.04.009 |
0.96 |
|
2019 |
Harris AM, Jacoby O, Remington RW, Travis SL, Mattingley JB. Taking a closer look at visual search: Just how feature-agnostic is singleton detection mode? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 30603988 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-018-01642-y |
0.96 |
|
2018 |
Remington RW, Burt JS, Becker SI. The curious case of spillover: Does it tell us much about saccade timing in reading? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 29968083 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-018-1544-5 |
0.68 |
|
2017 |
Harris AM, Remington RW. Contextual Cueing Improves Attentional Guidance, Even When Guidance Is Supposedly Optimal. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 28230395 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000394 |
0.96 |
|
2016 |
Vromen JM, Lipp OV, Remington RW, Becker SI. Threat captures attention, but not automatically: Top-down goals modulate attentional orienting to threat distractors. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 27234013 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-016-1142-3 |
0.68 |
|
2016 |
Retell JD, Becker SI, Remington RW. Previously seen and expected stimuli elicit surprise in the context of visual search. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 26742498 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-1052-9 |
1 |
|
2015 |
Remington RW, Yuen HW, Pashler H. With Practice, Keyboard Shortcuts Become Faster Than Menu Selection: A Crossover Interaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied. PMID 26651347 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000069 |
0.52 |
|
2015 |
Retell JD, Becker SI, Remington RW. An effective attentional set for a specific colour does not prevent capture by infrequently presented motion distractors. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 1-26. PMID 26299891 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1080738 |
1 |
|
2015 |
Garner KG, Matthews N, Remington RW, Dux PE. Transferability of Training Benefits Differs across Neural Events: Evidence from ERPs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1-16. PMID 26042504 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00833 |
1 |
|
2015 |
Folk CL, Remington RW. Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 41: 1153-65. PMID 26030438 DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000084 |
0.52 |
|
2015 |
Harris AM, Becker SI, Remington RW. Capture by colour: Evidence for dimension-specific singleton capture. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 26018643 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-0927-0 |
1 |
|
2015 |
Heathcote A, Loft S, Remington RW. Slow down and remember to remember! A delay theory of prospective memory costs. Psychological Review. 122: 376-410. PMID 25844878 DOI: 10.1037/a0038952 |
1 |
|
2015 |
Folk CL, Remington RW. Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 41: 1153-1165. DOI: 10.1037/xhp0000084 |
1 |
|
2014 |
Vromen JM, Lipp OV, Remington RW. The spider does not always win the fight for attention: Disengagement from threat is modulated by goal set. Cognition & Emotion. 1-12. PMID 25329044 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2014.969198 |
0.4 |
|
2014 |
Venini D, Remington RW, Horstmann G, Becker SI. Centre-of-Gravity Fixations in Visual Search: When Looking at Nothing Helps to Find Something. Journal of Ophthalmology. 2014: 237812. PMID 25002972 DOI: 10.1155/2014/237812 |
1 |
|
2014 |
Wu SC, Remington RW, Folk CL. Onsets do not override top-down goals, but they are responded to more quickly. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 76: 649-54. PMID 24596080 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-014-0637-z |
1 |
|
2014 |
Harrison WJ, Remington RW, Mattingley JB. Visual crowding is anisotropic along the horizontal meridian during smooth pursuit. Journal of Vision. 14. PMID 24453347 DOI: 10.1167/14.1.21 |
1 |
|
2013 |
Loft S, Smith RE, Remington RW. Minimizing the disruptive effects of prospective memory in simulated air traffic control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied. 19: 254-65. PMID 24059825 DOI: 10.1037/a0034141 |
1 |
|
2013 |
Harrison WJ, Mattingley JB, Remington RW. Author response. Releasing crowding prior to a saccade requires more than "attention": response to van Koningsbruggen and Buonocore. The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the Society For Neuroscience. 33: 4 pp following 11328. PMID 24003451 |
1 |
|
2013 |
Finlayson NJ, Remington RW, Retell JD, Grove PM. Segmentation by depth does not always facilitate visual search. Journal of Vision. 13. PMID 23847304 DOI: 10.1167/13.8.11 |
1 |
|
2013 |
Harris AM, Remington RW, Becker SI. Feature specificity in attentional capture by size and color. Journal of Vision. 13. PMID 23650630 DOI: 10.1167/13.3.12 |
1 |
|
2013 |
Harrison WJ, Retell JD, Remington RW, Mattingley JB. Visual crowding at a distance during predictive remapping. Current Biology : Cb. 23: 793-8. PMID 23562269 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.050 |
1 |
|
2013 |
Becker SI, Folk CL, Remington RW. Attentional capture does not depend on feature similarity, but on target-nontarget relations. Psychological Science. 24: 634-47. PMID 23558547 DOI: 10.1177/0956797612458528 |
1 |
|
2013 |
Irons JL, Remington RW. Can attentional control settings be maintained for two color-location conjunctions? Evidence from an RSVP task. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 75: 862-75. PMID 23444082 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-013-0439-8 |
1 |
|
2013 |
Harrison WJ, Mattingley JB, Remington RW. Eye movement targets are released from visual crowding. The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the Society For Neuroscience. 33: 2927-33. PMID 23407951 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4172-12.2013 |
1 |
|
2013 |
Loft S, Remington RW. Wait a second: Brief delays in responding reduce focality effects in event-based prospective memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 66: 1432-47. PMID 23281819 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2012.750677 |
1 |
|
2012 |
Harrison WJ, Mattingley JB, Remington RW. Pre-saccadic shifts of visual attention. Plos One. 7: e45670. PMID 23029175 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045670 |
1 |
|
2012 |
Finlayson NJ, Remington RW, Grove PM. The role of presentation method and depth singletons in visual search for objects moving in depth. Journal of Vision. 12: 13. PMID 22923724 DOI: 10.1167/12.8.13 |
1 |
|
2012 |
Irons JL, Folk CL, Remington RW. All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 38: 758-75. PMID 22201470 DOI: 10.1037/a0026578 |
1 |
|
2012 |
Remington R, Boehm-Davis D, Folk C. Introduction to Humans in Engineered Systems Introduction to Humans in Engineered Systems. DOI: 10.1002/9781118393758 |
1 |
|
2011 |
Campana E, Tanenhaus MK, Allen JF, Remington R. Natural discourse reference generation reduces cognitive load in spoken systems. Natural Language Engineering. 17: 311-329. PMID 25328423 DOI: 10.1017/S1351324910000227 |
1 |
|
2011 |
Loft S, Finnerty D, Remington RW. Using spatial context to support prospective memory in simulated air traffic control. Human Factors. 53: 662-71. PMID 22235528 DOI: 10.1177/0018720811421783 |
1 |
|
2011 |
Becker SI, Horstmann G, Remington RW. Perceptual grouping, not emotion, accounts for search asymmetries with schematic faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 37: 1739-57. PMID 21806311 DOI: 10.1037/a0024665 |
1 |
|
2011 |
Remington RW, Wu SC, Pashler H. What determines saccade timing in sequences of coordinated eye and hand movements? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 18: 538-43. PMID 21380589 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-011-0066-0 |
1 |
|
2011 |
Irons JL, Remington RW, McLean JP. Not so fast: Rethinking the effects of action video games on attentional capacity Australian Journal of Psychology. 63: 224-231. DOI: 10.1111/j.1742-9536.2011.00001.x |
1 |
|
2011 |
Loft S, Pearcy B, Remington RW. Varying the complexity of the prospective memory decision process in an air traffic control simulation Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology. 219: 77-84. DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000051 |
1 |
|
2010 |
Becker SI, Folk CL, Remington RW. The role of relational information in contingent capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 36: 1460-76. PMID 20919781 DOI: 10.1037/a0020370 |
1 |
|
2010 |
Loft S, Remington RW. Prospective memory and task interference in a continuous monitoring dynamic display task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied. 16: 145-57. PMID 20565199 DOI: 10.1037/a0018900 |
1 |
|
2010 |
Folk CL, Remington R. A critical evaluation of the disengagement hypothesis. Acta Psychologica. 135: 103-5; discussion 13. PMID 20510849 DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.012 |
1 |
|
2010 |
Ferri R, Drago V, Aricò D, Bruni O, Remington RW, Stamatakis K, Punjabi NM. The effects of experimental sleep fragmentation on cognitive processing. Sleep Medicine. 11: 378-85. PMID 20226732 DOI: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.01.006 |
1 |
|
2010 |
Ferri R, Drago V, Aricò D, Bruni O, Remington RW, Stamatakis K, Punjabi NM. Response to "The evidence that cyclic alternating pattern subtypes affect cognitive functioning is very weak" Sleep Medicine. 11: 803-804. DOI: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.05.001 |
1 |
|
2010 |
Lehmann S, Bolland S, Remington R, Humphreys MS, Neal A. Using A* graph traversal to model conflict resolution in air traffic control Proceedings of the 10th International Conference On Cognitive Modeling, Iccm 2010. 139-144. |
1 |
|
2009 |
Ruthruff E, Johnston JC, Remington RW. How strategic is the central bottleneck: can it be overcome by trying harder? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 35: 1368-84. PMID 19803643 DOI: 10.1037/a0015784 |
1 |
|
2009 |
Lachter J, Remington RW, Ruthruff E. Space, object, and task selection. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 71: 995-1014. PMID 19525533 DOI: 10.3758/APP.71.5.995 |
1 |
|
2009 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Wu SC. Additivity of abrupt onset effects supports nonspatial distraction, not the capture of spatial attention. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 71: 308-13. PMID 19304620 DOI: 10.3758/APP.71.2.308 |
1 |
|
2009 |
Boehm-Davis DA, Remington R. Reducing the disruptive effects of interruption: A cognitive framework for analysing the costs and benefits of intervention strategies Accident Analysis and Prevention. 41: 1124-1129. DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.06.029 |
1 |
|
2008 |
Lien MC, Ruthruff E, Goodin Z, Remington RW. Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: converging evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 34: 509-30. PMID 18505320 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.509 |
1 |
|
2008 |
Loft S, Kearney R, Remington R. Is task interference in event-based prospective memory dependent on cue presentation? Memory and Cognition. 36: 139-148. PMID 18323070 DOI: 10.3758/MC.36.1.139 |
1 |
|
2008 |
Folk CL, Remington RW. Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional control settings Visual Cognition. 16: 215-231. DOI: 10.1080/13506280701458804 |
1 |
|
2006 |
Lien MC, Allen PA, Ruthruff E, Grabbe J, McCann RS, Remington RW. Visual word recognition without central attention: evidence for greater automaticity with advancing age. Psychology and Aging. 21: 431-47. PMID 16953708 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.431 |
1 |
|
2006 |
Ruthruff E, Van Selst M, Johnston JC, Remington R. How does practice reduce dual-task interference: integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? Psychological Research. 70: 125-42. PMID 16703392 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-004-0192-7 |
1 |
|
2006 |
Hazeltine E, Ruthruff E, Remington RW. The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology. 52: 291-345. PMID 16581054 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.11.001 |
1 |
|
2006 |
Ruthruff E, Hazeltine E, Remington RW. What causes residual dual-task interference after practice? Psychological Research. 70: 494-503. PMID 16184395 DOI: 10.1007/s00426-005-0012-8 |
1 |
|
2006 |
Folk CL, Remington R. Top-down modulation of preattentive processing: Testing the recovery account of contingent capture Visual Cognition. 14: 445-465. DOI: 10.1080/13506280500193545 |
1 |
|
2005 |
Lien MC, Ruthruff E, Remington RW, Johnston JC. On the limits of advance preparation for a task switch: do people prepare all the task some of the time or some of the task all the time? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 31: 299-315. PMID 15826232 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.2.299 |
1 |
|
2005 |
Vera AH, John BE, Remington R, Matessa M, Freed MA. Automating human-performance modeling at the millisecond level Human-Computer Interaction. 20: 225-265. DOI: 10.1207/s15327051hci2003_1 |
1 |
|
2005 |
Gersh JR, McKneely JA, Remington RW. Cognitive engineering: Understanding human interaction with complex systems Johns Hopkins Apl Technical Digest (Applied Physics Laboratory). 26: 377-382. |
1 |
|
2005 |
Ravinder U, Remington RW, Lee SM. A reactive computational model of En-Route controller Conference Proceedings - Ieee International Conference On Systems, Man and Cybernetics. 2: 1628-1633. |
1 |
|
2005 |
Lee SM, Remington RW, Ravinder U. A framework for modeling and simulating human behavior in complex systems Conference Proceedings - Ieee International Conference On Systems, Man and Cybernetics. 4: 3161-3166. |
1 |
|
2005 |
Matessa M, Remington R. Eye movements in human performance modeling of space shuttle operations Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 1114-1118. |
1 |
|
2005 |
Matessa M, Remington R. Reusable templates of human performance in space shuttle procedures Conference Proceedings - Ieee International Conference On Systems, Man and Cybernetics. 2: 1610-1614. |
1 |
|
2004 |
Erev I, Barron G, Remington R. Right of way in the sky: Two problems in aircraft self-separation and the auction-based solution Human Factors. 46: 267-276. PMID 15359676 DOI: 10.1518/hfes.46.2.267.37339 |
1 |
|
2004 |
Miller CS, Remington RW. Modeling information navigation: Implications for information architecture Human-Computer Interaction. 19: 225-271. |
1 |
|
2004 |
Wu SC, Remington RW. Coordination of component mental operations in a multiple-response task Eye Tracking Research and Applications Symposium (Etra). 63-70. |
1 |
|
2003 |
Wu SC, Remington RW. Characteristics of covert and overt visual orienting: Evidence from attentional and oculomotor capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 29: 1050-67. PMID 14585022 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1050 |
1 |
|
2003 |
Ruthruff E, Johnston JC, Van Selst M, Whitsell S, Remington R. Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 29: 280-9. PMID 12760615 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.280 |
1 |
|
2002 |
Miller CS, Remington RW. Effects of structure and label ambiguity on information navigation Conference On Human Factors in Computing Systems - Proceedings. 630-631. |
1 |
|
2002 |
John B, Vera A, Matessa M, Freed M, Remington R. Automating CPM-GOMS Conference On Human Factors in Computing Systems - Proceedings. 4: 147-154. |
1 |
|
2001 |
Ruthruff E, Remington RW, Johnston JC. Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 27: 1404-19. PMID 11766933 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.6.1404 |
1 |
|
2001 |
Remington RW, Folk CL. A dissociation between attention and selection. Psychological Science. 12: 511-5. PMID 11760140 |
1 |
|
2001 |
Stone M, Dismukes K, Remington R. Prospective memory in dynamic environments: Effects of load, delay, and phonological rehearsal Memory. 9: 165-176. PMID 11469311 DOI: 10.1080/09658210143000100 |
1 |
|
2001 |
Remington RW, Folk CL, McLean JP. Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention? Perception & Psychophysics. 63: 298-307. PMID 11281104 |
1 |
|
2001 |
Folk C, Remington R, Lachter J, Ruthruff E. Top-down gating of preattentive featural processing in multidimensional objects Journal of Vision. 1: 111a. DOI: 10.1167/1.3.111 |
1 |
|
2000 |
Remington RW, Johnston JC, Ruthruff E, Gold M, Romera M. Visual search in complex displays: factors affecting conflict detection by air traffic controllers. Human Factors. 42: 349-66. PMID 11132797 |
1 |
|
2000 |
McCann RS, Remington RW, Van Selst M. A dual-task investigation of automaticity in visual word processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 26: 1352-70. PMID 10946719 |
1 |
|
1999 |
Folk CL, Remington R. Can new objects override attentional control settings? Perception & Psychophysics. 61: 727-39. PMID 10370339 |
1 |
|
1998 |
Folk CL, Remington R. Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 24: 847-58. PMID 9627420 |
1 |
|
1997 |
Freed M, Remington R. Managing decision resources in plan execution Ijcai International Joint Conference On Artificial Intelligence. 1: 322-326. |
1 |
|
1996 |
Hanna A, Remington R. The representation of color and form in long-term memory Memory and Cognition. 24: 322-330. PMID 8718766 |
1 |
|
1994 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Wright JH. The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 20: 317-29. PMID 8189195 |
1 |
|
1993 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Johnston JC. Contingent Attentional Capture: A Reply to Yantis (1993) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 19: 682-685. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.3.682 |
1 |
|
1992 |
Remington RW, Johnston JC, Yantis S. Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets. Perception & Psychophysics. 51: 279-90. PMID 1561053 DOI: 10.3758/BF03212254 |
1 |
|
1992 |
Folk CL, Remington RW, Johnston JC. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 18: 1030-44. PMID 1431742 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030 |
1 |
|
1989 |
Weiss RS, Remington R, Ellis SR. Sampling distributions of the entropy in visual scanning Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 21: 348-352. DOI: 10.3758/BF03202796 |
1 |
|
1986 |
Remington R, Williams D. ON THE SELECTION AND EVALUATION OF VISUAL DISPLAY SYMBOLOGY: FACTORS INFLUENCING SEARCH AND IDENTIFICATION TIMES Human Factors. 28: 407-420. PMID 3793109 |
1 |
|
1984 |
Remington R, Pierce L. Moving attention: Evidence for time-invariant shifts of visual selective attention Perception & Psychophysics. 35: 393-399. PMID 6739275 DOI: 10.3758/BF03206344 |
1 |
|
1984 |
Remington R, Williams D. EVALUATION OF DISPLAY SYMBOLOGY: A CHRONOMETRIC STUDY OF VISUAL SEARCH Aiaa Paper. 84-89. |
1 |
|
1980 |
Remington RW. Attention and saccadic eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 6: 726-44. PMID 6449540 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.6.4.726 |
1 |
|
1979 |
Shulman GL, Remington RW, McLean JP. Moving attention through visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 5: 522-6. PMID 528957 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.5.3.522 |
1 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |