Year |
Citation |
Score |
2019 |
Rogers B, Anstis S, Ashida H, Kitaoka A. Reversed Phi and the "Phenomenal Phenomena" Revisited. I-Perception. 10: 2041669519856906. PMID 31384414 DOI: 10.1177/2041669519856906 |
0.584 |
|
2019 |
Rogers B. Where Have All the Illusions Gone? Perception. 48: 193-196. PMID 30727833 DOI: 10.1177/0301006619828117 |
0.305 |
|
2016 |
Rogers B. The Effectiveness of Vertical Perspective and Pursuit Eye Movements for Disambiguating Motion Parallax Transformations. Perception. PMID 27343187 DOI: 10.1177/0301006616655815 |
0.392 |
|
2016 |
Rogers B. Revisiting Motion Parallax as a Source of 3-D Information. Perception. PMID 27343185 DOI: 10.1177/0301006616655816 |
0.391 |
|
2016 |
Rogers B, Naumenko O. Perception of straightness and parallelism with minimal distance information. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 27025213 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-016-1083-x |
0.35 |
|
2015 |
Rogers B, Naumenko O. The new moon illusion and the role of perspective in the perception of straight and parallel lines. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 77: 249-57. PMID 25239097 DOI: 10.3758/s13414-014-0767-3 |
0.313 |
|
2012 |
Anstis S, Rogers B. Binocular fusion of luminance, color, motion and flicker--two eyes are worse than one. Vision Research. 53: 47-53. PMID 22138529 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.11.005 |
0.559 |
|
2012 |
Rogers B. Motion parallax, pursuit eye movements and the assumption of stationarity Journal of Vision. 12: 251-251. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.251 |
0.345 |
|
2011 |
Anstis S, Rogers B. Illusory rotation of a spoked wheel. I-Perception. 2: 720-3. PMID 23145255 DOI: 10.1068/i0483 |
0.576 |
|
2011 |
Rogers B. Perspective transformations and depth scaling in stereopsis and motion parallax Journal of Vision. 11: 58-58. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.58 |
0.329 |
|
2011 |
Rogers B, Gillam B. The induced effect revisited: Discounting the effects of head orientation Perception. 40: 7-7. DOI: 10.1068/V110175 |
0.589 |
|
2010 |
Rogers B, Gyani A. Binocular disparities, motion parallax, and geometric perspective in patrick hughes's 'reverspectives': Theoretical analysis and empirical findings Perception. 39: 330-348. PMID 20465170 DOI: 10.1068/p6583 |
0.418 |
|
2010 |
Rogers B, Colam C, Cant C. Sensitivity to disparity modulations in ground plane surfaces Journal of Vision. 8: 1087-1087. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.1087 |
0.317 |
|
2010 |
Anstis S, Rogers B. Binocular fusion can make two eyes worse than one Journal of Vision. 7: 801-801. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.801 |
0.533 |
|
2010 |
Rogers B. Interactions between disparity, parallax and perspective: Linking 'Reverspectives', hollow masks and the apparent motion seen in random dot stereograms Journal of Vision. 10: 55-55. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.55 |
0.333 |
|
2009 |
Rogers B, Rogers C. Visual globes, celestial spheres, and the perception of straight and parallel lines Perception. 38: 1295-1312. PMID 19911628 DOI: 10.1068/p6328 |
0.395 |
|
2009 |
Howard IP, Rogers BJ. Seeing in Depth Seeing in Depth. 1: 1-1301. DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195367607.001.0001 |
0.51 |
|
2009 |
Rogers B, Graham M. Motion parallax as an independent cue for depth perception Perception. 38: 125-134. DOI: 10.1068/P080125 |
0.412 |
|
2008 |
Howard IP, Rogers BJ. Binocular Vision and Stereopsis Binocular Vision and Stereopsis. 1-746. DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195084764.001.0001 |
0.556 |
|
2007 |
Rogers B, Brecher K. Straight lines, 'uncurved lines', and Helmholtz's 'great circles on the celestial sphere'. Perception. 36: 1275-89. PMID 18196696 DOI: 10.1068/p5652 |
0.342 |
|
2006 |
Groeger J, Harris J, Watt S, Rose D, Hibbard P, Riddell T, Glennerster A, Rogers B, Langley K, Rushton S. Professor Markland Frederick Bradshaw 1961–2004 Spatial Vision. 19: 1-8. DOI: 10.1163/156856806775009241 |
0.499 |
|
2004 |
Rogers BJ, Ambler M. Vertical disparities can recalibrate the vergence system Journal of Vision. 4: 171-171. DOI: 10.1167/4.8.171 |
0.353 |
|
2003 |
Allison RS, Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Geometric and induced effects in binocular stereopsis and motion parallax. Vision Research. 43: 1879-93. PMID 12826111 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00298-0 |
0.719 |
|
2003 |
Rogers BJ. Depth and size scaling created by the differential perspective of ground plane surfaces Journal of Vision. 3: 77a. DOI: 10.1167/3.9.77 |
0.324 |
|
2002 |
Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF, Langley K, Rogers BJ. The stereoscopic anisotropy: individual differences and underlying mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 28: 469-76. PMID 11999867 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.469 |
0.731 |
|
2002 |
Rogers B, Anstis S. After-effects of expansion: No evidence for a change-of-size mechanism Journal of Vision. 2: 738a. DOI: 10.1167/2.7.738 |
0.516 |
|
2002 |
Allison RS, Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Induced effects in motion parallax Journal of Vision. 2: 661a. DOI: 10.1167/2.7.661 |
0.686 |
|
1999 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ. Sensitivity to horizontal and vertical corrugations defined by binocular disparity. Vision Research. 39: 3049-56. PMID 10664803 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00015-2 |
0.648 |
|
1999 |
Ledgeway T, Rogers BJ. The effects of eccentricity and vergence angle upon the relative tilt of corresponding vertical and horizontal meridia revealed using the minimum motion paradigm. Perception. 28: 143-53. PMID 10615456 DOI: 10.1068/P2738 |
0.448 |
|
1999 |
Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Disparity minimisation, cyclovergence, and the validity of nonius lines as a technique for measuring torsional alignment. Perception. 28: 127-41. PMID 10615455 DOI: 10.1068/P2778 |
0.648 |
|
1998 |
Prince SJ, Rogers BJ. Sensitivity to disparity corrugations in peripheral vision. Vision Research. 38: 2533-7. PMID 12116701 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00118-7 |
0.545 |
|
1998 |
Glennerster A, Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Cues to viewing distance for stereoscopic depth constancy. Perception. 27: 1357-65. PMID 10505180 DOI: 10.1068/P271357 |
0.634 |
|
1998 |
Prince SJ, Eagle RA, Rogers BJ. Contrast masking reveals spatial-frequency channels in stereopsis. Perception. 27: 1345-55. PMID 10505179 DOI: 10.1068/P271345 |
0.533 |
|
1998 |
Allison RS, Howard IP, Rogers BJ, Bridge H. Temporal aspects of slant and inclination perception. Perception. 27: 1287-304. PMID 10505175 DOI: 10.1068/P271287 |
0.696 |
|
1997 |
Eagle RA, Rogers BJ. Effects of dot density, patch size and contrast on the upper spatial limit for direction discrimination in random-dot kinematograms. Vision Research. 37: 2091-102. PMID 9327057 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00153-8 |
0.37 |
|
1997 |
Lee B, Rogers B. Disparity modulation sensitivity for narrow-band-filtered stereograms Vision Research. 37: 1769-1777. PMID 9274764 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00274-X |
0.336 |
|
1996 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ. The interaction of binocular disparity and motion parallax in the computation of depth. Vision Research. 36: 3457-68. PMID 8977012 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00072-7 |
0.67 |
|
1996 |
Glennerster A, Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Stereoscopic depth constancy depends on the subject's task. Vision Research. 36: 3441-56. PMID 8977011 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00090-9 |
0.629 |
|
1996 |
Eagle RA, Rogers BJ. Motion detection is limited by element density not spatial frequency. Vision Research. 36: 545-58. PMID 8855000 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)89252-2 |
0.336 |
|
1996 |
Bradshaw MF, Glennerster A, Rogers BJ. The effect of display size on disparity scaling from differential perspective and vergence cues. Vision Research. 36: 1255-64. PMID 8711905 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00190-5 |
0.639 |
|
1996 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ. Do Monocular Tilt and Spatial Frequency Aftereffects Induce the Binocular Perception of Inclination and Slant? Perception. 25: 18-18. DOI: 10.1068/V96P0201 |
0.623 |
|
1996 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ. Do Monocular Tilt and Spatial Frequency Aftereffects Induce the Binocular Perception of Inclination and Slant? Perception. 25: 21-21. DOI: 10.1068/v96l0912 |
0.623 |
|
1996 |
Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Assessing the Role of Vergence Changes in the Perception of Random-Dot Stereograms by Using Open-Loop Control of Vergence Perception. 25: 131-131. DOI: 10.1068/V96L0911 |
0.548 |
|
1996 |
Swash SA, Rogers BJ. The perceived rotation of motion parallax surfaces: Dependence on spatial frequency of depth corrugations Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science. 37: S286. |
0.306 |
|
1995 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ, De Bruyn B. Perceptual latency and complex random-dot stereograms. Perception. 24: 749-59. PMID 8710438 DOI: 10.1068/P240749 |
0.633 |
|
1995 |
Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Disparity scaling and the perception of frontoparallel surfaces. Perception. 24: 155-79. PMID 7617423 DOI: 10.1068/P240155 |
0.667 |
|
1993 |
Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Vertical disparities, differential perspective and binocular stereopsis. Nature. 361: 253-5. PMID 8423851 DOI: 10.1038/361253a0 |
0.686 |
|
1993 |
Cagenello R, Rogers BJ. Anisotropies in the perception of stereoscopic surfaces: the role of orientation disparity. Vision Research. 33: 2189-201. PMID 8273286 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90099-I |
0.398 |
|
1993 |
Glennerster A, Rogers BJ. New depth to the Müller-Lyer illusion. Perception. 22: 691-704. PMID 8255699 DOI: 10.1068/p220691 |
0.358 |
|
1992 |
Rogers S, Rogers BJ. Visual and nonvisual information disambiguate surfaces specified by motion parallax. Perception & Psychophysics. 52: 446-52. PMID 1437477 DOI: 10.3758/BF03206704 |
0.394 |
|
1991 |
Rogers BJ. Visual perception. Why the eye doesn't shape up. Nature. 349: 365-6. PMID 1992337 DOI: 10.1038/349365a0 |
0.325 |
|
1991 |
Gillam B, Rogers B. Orientation disparity, deformation, and stereoscopic slant perception Perception. 20: 441-448. PMID 1771129 DOI: 10.1068/P200441 |
0.602 |
|
1989 |
Rogers B, Cagenello R. Disparity curvature and the perception of three-dimensional surfaces Nature. 339: 135-137. PMID 2716833 DOI: 10.1038/339135A0 |
0.427 |
|
1989 |
Rogers BJ, Collett TS. The appearance of surfaces specified by motion parallax and binocular disparity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. a, Human Experimental Psychology. 41: 697-717. PMID 2587795 DOI: 10.1080/14640748908402390 |
0.459 |
|
1986 |
Rogers B, Koenderink J. Monocular aniseikonia: A motion parallax analogue of the disparity-induced effect Nature. 322: 62-63. PMID 3724851 DOI: 10.1038/322062A0 |
0.423 |
|
1986 |
Anstis SM, Rogers BJ. Illusory continuous motion from oscillating positive-negative patterns: implications for motion perception. Perception. 15: 627-40. PMID 3588223 DOI: 10.1068/P150627 |
0.588 |
|
1983 |
Rogers BJ, Graham ME. Anisotropies in the perception of three-dimensional surfaces. Science (New York, N.Y.). 221: 1409-11. PMID 6612351 DOI: 10.1126/Science.6612351 |
0.408 |
|
1982 |
Graham M, Rogers B. Simultaneous and successive contrast effects in the perception of depth from motion-parallax and stereoscopic information Perception. 11: 247-262. PMID 7167333 DOI: 10.1068/P110247 |
0.411 |
|
1982 |
Rogers B, Graham M. Similarities between motion parallax and stereopsis in human depth perception Vision Research. 22: 261-270. PMID 7101762 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90126-2 |
0.432 |
|
1978 |
Anstis S, Rogers B, Henry J. Interactions between simultaneous contrast and coloured afterimages. Vision Research. 18: 899-911. PMID 706164 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(78)90016-0 |
0.517 |
|
1978 |
Anstis SM, Howard IP, Rogers B. A Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet illusion for visual depth. Vision Research. 18: 213-7. PMID 664293 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(78)90189-X |
0.665 |
|
1975 |
Anstis SM, Rogers BJ. Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast. Vision Research. 15: 957-61. PMID 1166630 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(75)90236-9 |
0.597 |
|
1975 |
Rogers BJ, Anstis SM. Reversed depth from positive and negative stereograms Perception. 4: 193-201. DOI: 10.1068/P040193 |
0.562 |
|
1972 |
Rogers BJ, Anstis SM. Intensity versus adaptation and the Pulfrich stereophenomenon. Vision Research. 12: 909-28. PMID 5037709 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(72)90014-4 |
0.511 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |