Year |
Citation |
Score |
2006 |
Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF. 2-D tilt and 3-D slant illusions in perception and action tasks. Perception. 35: 1297-305. PMID 17214377 DOI: 10.1068/p5365 |
0.568 |
|
2006 |
Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB, Parton AD, Rose D, Langley K. Surface orientation, modulation frequency and the detection and perception of depth defined by binocular disparity and motion parallax. Vision Research. 46: 2636-44. PMID 16571356 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2006.02.011 |
0.786 |
|
2006 |
Bradshaw MF, Glennerster A. Stereoscopic acuity and observation distance. Spatial Vision. 19: 21-36. PMID 16411481 |
0.3 |
|
2005 |
Clarke TJ, Bradshaw MF, Field DT, Hampson SE, Rose D. The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception. 34: 1171-80. PMID 16309112 DOI: 10.1068/P5203 |
0.724 |
|
2004 |
Bradshaw MF, Elliott KM, Watt SJ, Hibbard PB, Davies IR, Simpson PJ. Binocular cues and the control of prehension. Spatial Vision. 17: 95-110. PMID 15078014 DOI: 10.1163/156856804322778288 |
0.685 |
|
2004 |
Clarke T, Bradshaw M, Field D, Rose D. Children's perception of emotion from point-light displays Perception. 33: 0-0. DOI: 10.1068/V040144 |
0.717 |
|
2003 |
Rose D, Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB. Attention affects the stereoscopic depth aftereffect. Perception. 32: 635-40. PMID 12854650 DOI: 10.1068/P3324 |
0.739 |
|
2003 |
Allison RS, Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Geometric and induced effects in binocular stereopsis and motion parallax. Vision Research. 43: 1879-93. PMID 12826111 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00298-0 |
0.665 |
|
2003 |
Watt SJ, Bradshaw MF. The visual control of reaching and grasping: binocular disparity and motion parallax. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 29: 404-15. PMID 12760624 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.404 |
0.348 |
|
2003 |
Barrett DJ, Bradshaw MF, Rose D. Endogenous shifts of covert attention operate within multiple coordinate frames: evidence from a feature-priming task. Perception. 32: 41-52. PMID 12613785 DOI: 10.1068/P3298 |
0.773 |
|
2003 |
Watt SJ, Bradshaw MF, Clarke TJ, Elliot KM. Binocular vision and prehension in middle childhood. Neuropsychologia. 41: 415-20. PMID 12559159 DOI: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00174-4 |
0.656 |
|
2003 |
Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF. Reaching for virtual objects: binocular disparity and the control of prehension. Experimental Brain Research. 148: 196-201. PMID 12520407 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-002-1295-2 |
0.583 |
|
2003 |
Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB. Reaching for virtual objects: Binocular disparity retinal motion and the control of prehension Arquivos Brasileiros De Oftalmologia. 66: 53-61. |
0.58 |
|
2002 |
Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB, van der Willigen R, Watt SJ, Simpson PJ. The stereoscopic anisotropy affects manual pointing. Spatial Vision. 15: 443-58. PMID 12434823 DOI: 10.1163/156856802320401900 |
0.615 |
|
2002 |
Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF, Langley K, Rogers BJ. The stereoscopic anisotropy: individual differences and underlying mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 28: 469-76. PMID 11999867 DOI: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.2.469 |
0.732 |
|
2002 |
Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF. Isotropic integration of binocular disparity and relative motion in the perception of three-dimensional shape. Spatial Vision. 15: 205-17. PMID 11991575 DOI: 10.1163/15685680252875174 |
0.63 |
|
2002 |
Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB, Gillam B. Perceptual latencies to discriminate surface orientation in stereopsis. Perception & Psychophysics. 64: 32-40. PMID 11916300 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03194555 |
0.702 |
|
2002 |
Allison RS, Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Induced effects in motion parallax Journal of Vision. 2: 661a. DOI: 10.1167/2.7.661 |
0.643 |
|
2002 |
Clarke TJ, Bradshaw MF, Hampson SE. The importance of temporal coherence in the perception of natural communication behaviours Journal of Vision. 2. DOI: 10.1167/2.7.350 |
0.675 |
|
2002 |
Rose D, Bradshaw M. Attention, vision and action Spatial Vision. 15: 115-116. DOI: 10.1163/15685680252875101 |
0.611 |
|
2002 |
Bradshaw MF, Elliott KM, Watt SJ, Davies IRL, Willis A. Do observers exploit binocular disparity information in motor tasks within telepresence environments? Proceedings of Spie - the International Society For Optical Engineering. 4660: 331-342. DOI: 10.1117/12.468048 |
0.53 |
|
2001 |
Barrett DJ, Bradshaw MF, Rose D, Everatt J, Simpson PJ. Reflexive shifts of covert attention operate in an egocentric coordinate frame. Perception. 30: 1083-91. PMID 11694084 DOI: 10.1068/P3165 |
0.774 |
|
2001 |
Bradshaw MF, Parton AD, Glennerster A. The task-dependent use of binocular disparity and motion parallax information. Vision Research. 40: 3725-34. PMID 11090665 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00214-5 |
0.323 |
|
2001 |
Willis AR, Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB, Stringer N, Davies IRL, Watt SJ. Can telepresent observers learn to take account of enhanced binocular disparities? Proceedings of Spie - the International Society For Optical Engineering. 4297: 346-354. DOI: 10.1117/12.430834 |
0.651 |
|
2001 |
Watt SJ, Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB, Davies IRL, Stringer N, Willis AR. Can observers exploit enhanced motion parallax to control reaching movements within telepresence environments ? Proceedings of Spie - the International Society For Optical Engineering. 4299: 429-438. DOI: 10.1117/12.429513 |
0.669 |
|
2000 |
Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF, Eagle RA. Cue combination in the motion correspondence problem. Proceedings. Biological Sciences / the Royal Society. 267: 1369-74. PMID 10972134 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1152 |
0.613 |
|
2000 |
Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB, van der Willigen R, Watt SJ, Davies IRL, Stringer N, Beagley N, Willis A. Can observers exploit enhanced disparity information to control reaching movements within telepresence environments? Proceedings of Spie - the International Society For Optical Engineering. 3957: 253-261. DOI: 10.1117/12.384450 |
0.675 |
|
2000 |
Langley K, Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB. Total least squares: a general tool to explain motion and tilt aftereffects Perception. 29: 0-0. DOI: 10.1068/V000232 |
0.513 |
|
1999 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ. Sensitivity to horizontal and vertical corrugations defined by binocular disparity. Vision Research. 39: 3049-56. PMID 10664803 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00015-2 |
0.537 |
|
1999 |
Everatt J, Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB. Visual processing and dyslexia. Perception. 28: 243-54. PMID 10615463 |
0.609 |
|
1999 |
Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF. Does binocular disparity facilitate the detection of transparent motion? Perception. 28: 183-91. PMID 10615459 |
0.608 |
|
1999 |
Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Disparity minimisation, cyclovergence, and the validity of nonius lines as a technique for measuring torsional alignment. Perception. 28: 127-41. PMID 10615455 DOI: 10.1068/P2778 |
0.518 |
|
1999 |
Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF, DeBruyn B. Global motion processing is not tuned for binocular disparity. Vision Research. 39: 961-74. PMID 10341948 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00155-2 |
0.614 |
|
1998 |
Glennerster A, Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Cues to viewing distance for stereoscopic depth constancy. Perception. 27: 1357-65. PMID 10505180 DOI: 10.1068/P271357 |
0.564 |
|
1998 |
Bradshaw MF, Parton AD, Eagle RA. The interaction of binocular disparity and motion parallax in determining perceived depth and perceived size. Perception. 27: 1317-31. PMID 10505177 DOI: 10.1068/P271317 |
0.329 |
|
1998 |
Bradshaw MF, Hibbard PB. Depth perception. Perception. 27: 1263-5. PMID 10505173 |
0.568 |
|
1997 |
Bradshaw MF, Cumming BG. The direction of retinal motion facilitates binocular stereopsis. Proceedings. Biological Sciences / the Royal Society. 264: 1421-7. PMID 9364782 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0198 |
0.378 |
|
1997 |
Bradshaw MF, De Bruyn B, Eagle RA, Parton AD. The Task-Dependent use of Binocular Disparity and Motion Parallax under Natural Viewing Conditions Perception. 26: 99-99. DOI: 10.1068/V970186 |
0.319 |
|
1997 |
Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF, Gillam B. Differences in Processing Time for Stereoscopic Inclination and Slant Perception. 26: 173-173. DOI: 10.1068/V970182 |
0.636 |
|
1997 |
Hegarty MR, Hibbard PB, Bradshaw MF, De Bruyn B, Parton AD. Anisotropic Temporal Integration in the Perception of Stereoscopic Corrugations Perception. 26: 170-170. DOI: 10.1068/V970178 |
0.541 |
|
1996 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ. The interaction of binocular disparity and motion parallax in the computation of depth. Vision Research. 36: 3457-68. PMID 8977012 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00072-7 |
0.607 |
|
1996 |
Glennerster A, Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Stereoscopic depth constancy depends on the subject's task. Vision Research. 36: 3441-56. PMID 8977011 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00090-9 |
0.55 |
|
1996 |
Bradshaw MF, Glennerster A, Rogers BJ. The effect of display size on disparity scaling from differential perspective and vergence cues. Vision Research. 36: 1255-64. PMID 8711905 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00190-5 |
0.519 |
|
1996 |
Parton AD, Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ, Davies IRL. The Effect of Surface Orientation on the Perception of Stereoscopic Corrugations Perception. 25: 117-117. DOI: 10.1068/V96P0210 |
0.574 |
|
1996 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ. Do Monocular Tilt and Spatial Frequency Aftereffects Induce the Binocular Perception of Inclination and Slant? Perception. 25: 18-18. DOI: 10.1068/V96P0201 |
0.534 |
|
1996 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ. Do Monocular Tilt and Spatial Frequency Aftereffects Induce the Binocular Perception of Inclination and Slant? Perception. 25: 21-21. DOI: 10.1068/v96l0912 |
0.534 |
|
1996 |
Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Assessing the Role of Vergence Changes in the Perception of Random-Dot Stereograms by Using Open-Loop Control of Vergence Perception. 25: 131-131. DOI: 10.1068/V96L0911 |
0.453 |
|
1995 |
Bradshaw MF, Rogers BJ, De Bruyn B. Perceptual latency and complex random-dot stereograms. Perception. 24: 749-59. PMID 8710438 DOI: 10.1068/P240749 |
0.532 |
|
1995 |
Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Disparity scaling and the perception of frontoparallel surfaces. Perception. 24: 155-79. PMID 7617423 DOI: 10.1068/P240155 |
0.556 |
|
1993 |
Rogers BJ, Bradshaw MF. Vertical disparities, differential perspective and binocular stereopsis. Nature. 361: 253-5. PMID 8423851 DOI: 10.1038/361253a0 |
0.58 |
|
1987 |
Bradshaw MF, Frisby JP, Mayhew JE. The recovery of structure from motion: no evidence for a special link with the convergent disparity mechanism. Perception. 16: 351-7. PMID 3432030 DOI: 10.1068/p160351 |
0.689 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |