Year |
Citation |
Score |
2023 |
Caziot B, Rolfs M, Backus BT. Orienting attention across binocular disparity. Pnas Nexus. 2: pgad314. PMID 37822768 DOI: 10.1093/pnasnexus/pgad314 |
0.33 |
|
2019 |
Cai LT, Yuan AE, Backus BT. Binocular global motion perception is improved by dichoptic segregation when stimuli have high contrast and high speed. Journal of Vision. 19: 10. PMID 31722005 DOI: 10.1167/19.13.10 |
0.541 |
|
2018 |
Cai LT, Backus B. Global motion perception is faster but less accurate with dark random-dot kinematograms Journal of Vision. 18: 295. DOI: 10.1167/18.10.295 |
0.465 |
|
2017 |
Caziot B, Backus BT, Lin E. Early dynamics of stereoscopic surface slant perception. Journal of Vision. 17: 4. PMID 29196763 DOI: 10.1167/17.14.4 |
0.358 |
|
2017 |
Cai L, Backus B. Effects of contrast polarity and binocularity on global motion discrimination Journal of Vision. 17: 420. DOI: 10.1167/17.10.420 |
0.478 |
|
2016 |
Cai LT, Yuan A, Backus B. Monocular gain control explains dichoptic benefit in binocular global motion perception Journal of Vision. 16: 427. DOI: 10.1167/16.12.427 |
0.479 |
|
2015 |
Cai LT, Yuan A, Backus B. Interactions among contrast, spatial displacement, and dichoptic viewing during binocular combination in global motion perception. Journal of Vision. 15: 270. PMID 26325958 DOI: 10.1167/15.12.270 |
0.548 |
|
2014 |
Jain A, Fuller S, Backus BT. Cue-recruitment for extrinsic signals after training with low information stimuli. Plos One. 9: e96383. PMID 24804788 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0096383 |
0.672 |
|
2014 |
Harrison SJ, Backus BT. A trained perceptual bias that lasts for weeks. Vision Research. 99: 148-53. PMID 24631663 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.03.001 |
0.537 |
|
2013 |
Jain A, Backus BT. Generalization of cue recruitment to non-moving stimuli: location and surface-texture contingent biases for 3-D shape perception. Vision Research. 82: 13-21. PMID 23438583 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2013.02.006 |
0.684 |
|
2013 |
Adams-Bedford J, Wallis G, Backus B. The impact of intention, action, and learnt contingency on visual perception. Journal of Vision. 13: 754-754. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.754 |
0.344 |
|
2013 |
Llerena Law C, Backus B, Yuan A, Natanelova O, Steele L, Tseng I, Cai L. Use of dichoptic random dot kinematograms to assess amblyopic suppression Journal of Vision. 13: 544-544. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.544 |
0.418 |
|
2012 |
Harrison SJ, Backus BT. Associative learning of shape as a cue to appearance: a new demonstration of cue recruitment. Journal of Vision. 12. PMID 22427695 DOI: 10.1167/12.3.15 |
0.614 |
|
2012 |
Backus BT. Recruitment of New Visual Cues for Perceptual Appearance Sensory Cue Integration. DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195387247.003.0006 |
0.308 |
|
2011 |
Harrison SJ, Backus BT, Jain A. Disambiguation of Necker cube rotation by monocular and binocular depth cues: relative effectiveness for establishing long-term bias. Vision Research. 51: 978-86. PMID 21335023 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2011.02.011 |
0.728 |
|
2010 |
Jain A, Backus BT. Experience affects the use of ego-motion signals during 3D shape perception. Journal of Vision. 10. PMID 21191132 DOI: 10.1167/10.14.30 |
0.654 |
|
2010 |
Jain A, Fuller S, Backus BT. Absence of cue-recruitment for extrinsic signals: sounds, spots, and swirling dots fail to influence perceived 3D rotation direction after training. Plos One. 5: e13295. PMID 20949047 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0013295 |
0.688 |
|
2010 |
Di Luca M, Ernst MO, Backus BT. Learning to use an invisible visual signal for perception. Current Biology : Cb. 20: 1860-3. PMID 20933421 DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.047 |
0.683 |
|
2010 |
Harrison S, Backus B. Disambiguating Necker cube rotation using a location cue: what types of spatial location signal can the visual system learn? Journal of Vision. 10: 23. PMID 20884572 DOI: 10.1167/10.6.23 |
0.633 |
|
2010 |
Harrison SJ, Backus BT. Uninformative visual experience establishes long term perceptual bias. Vision Research. 50: 1905-11. PMID 20600232 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.06.013 |
0.327 |
|
2010 |
Jain A, Backus B. Cross-modal perceptual learning is non-trivial: synchronous and semantically plausible auditory cues were not recruited for a visual appearance task Journal of Vision. 9: 875-875. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.875 |
0.669 |
|
2010 |
Meade R, Backus B, Haijiang Q. Cue probability learning by the human perceptual system Journal of Vision. 9: 42-42. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.42 |
0.351 |
|
2010 |
Fuller S, Backus B, Dam Lv, Ernst M. Short-term dynamics of perceptual bias for bistable stimuli Journal of Vision. 9: 41-41. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.41 |
0.539 |
|
2010 |
Ernst MO, DiLuca M, Backus B. Recruitment of an invisible depth cue Journal of Vision. 9: 34-34. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.34 |
0.6 |
|
2010 |
Chajka K, Backus B, Wilmer J. Ability to use stereo predicts recruitment of a correlated artificial cue Journal of Vision. 9: 283-283. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.283 |
0.634 |
|
2010 |
Wilmer J, Backus B. Behavioral genetic evidence for plasticity in the oculomotor system Journal of Vision. 8: 633-633. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.633 |
0.573 |
|
2010 |
Backus B. Bayesian model of cue combination for ambiguous stimuli Journal of Vision. 7: 66-66. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.66 |
0.379 |
|
2010 |
Haijiang Q, Backus B. Newly recruited cue trades against pre-existing cues during the construction of visual appearance Journal of Vision. 7: 65-65. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.65 |
0.425 |
|
2010 |
Haijiang Q, Backus BT. Temporal aspects of cue recruitment in visual perception Journal of Vision. 6: 169-169. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.169 |
0.309 |
|
2010 |
Backus BT, Hillis JM, Frumkin J, Saunders JA. Early temporal dynamics of cue combination for slant from stereo and texture Journal of Vision. 5: 400-400. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.400 |
0.694 |
|
2010 |
van Dam L, Ernst M, Backus B. Pre-exposure interferes with perceptual learning for ambiguous stimuli Journal of Vision. 10: 1140-1140. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.1140 |
0.556 |
|
2009 |
Wilmer JB, Backus BT. Genetic and environmental contributions to strabismus and phoria: evidence from twins. Vision Research. 49: 2485-93. PMID 19679148 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.006 |
0.563 |
|
2009 |
Backus BT. The Mixture of Bernoulli Experts: a theory to quantify reliance on cues in dichotomous perceptual decisions. Journal of Vision. 9: 6.1-19. PMID 19271876 DOI: 10.1167/9.1.6 |
0.361 |
|
2009 |
Backus B. Recruitment of new cues for visual perception: A progress report Journal of Vision. 9: 31-31. DOI: 10.1167/9.14.31 |
0.408 |
|
2008 |
Wilmer JB, Backus BT. Self-reported Magic Eye stereogram skill predicts stereoacuity. Perception. 37: 1297-300. PMID 18853564 DOI: 10.1068/p6081 |
0.603 |
|
2007 |
Saunders JA, Backus BT. Both parallelism and orthogonality are used to perceive 3D slant of rectangles from 2D images. Journal of Vision. 7: 7. PMID 17685790 DOI: 10.1167/7.6.7 |
0.498 |
|
2007 |
Backus BT, Haijiang Q. Competition between newly recruited and pre-existing visual cues during the construction of visual appearance. Vision Research. 47: 919-24. PMID 17303207 DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.12.008 |
0.368 |
|
2007 |
Wilmer J, Backus B. Precision of depth judgement from binocular disparity is heritable Journal of Vision. 7: 831-831. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.831 |
0.653 |
|
2006 |
Saunders JA, Backus BT. The accuracy and reliability of perceived depth from linear perspective as a function of image size. Journal of Vision. 6: 933-54. PMID 17083286 DOI: 10.1167/6.9.7 |
0.509 |
|
2006 |
Saunders JA, Backus BT. Perception of surface slant from oriented textures. Journal of Vision. 6: 882-97. PMID 17083282 DOI: 10.1167/6.9.3 |
0.569 |
|
2006 |
Haijiang Q, Saunders JA, Stone RW, Backus BT. Demonstration of cue recruitment: change in visual appearance by means of Pavlovian conditioning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 103: 483-8. PMID 16387858 DOI: 10.1073/Pnas.0506728103 |
0.629 |
|
2006 |
Duke PA, Oruç I, Qi H, Backus BT. Depth aftereffects mediated by vertical disparities: evidence for vertical disparity driven calibration of extraretinal signals during stereopsis. Vision Research. 46: 228-41. PMID 16257031 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2005.09.004 |
0.588 |
|
2005 |
Saunders JA, Backus BT. Perception of slant-from-texture for textures with oriented symmetry Journal of Vision. 5: 996-996. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.996 |
0.515 |
|
2005 |
Qi H, Backus BT, Stone RW, Saunders JA, Marshall DA. Recruitment of new perceptual cues Journal of Vision. 5: 863-863. DOI: 10.1167/5.8.863 |
0.662 |
|
2004 |
Zabulis X, Backus BT. Starry night: a texture devoid of depth cues. Journal of the Optical Society of America. a, Optics, Image Science, and Vision. 21: 2049-60. PMID 15535362 DOI: 10.1364/JOSAA.21.002049 |
0.35 |
|
2004 |
Qi H, Backus BT. Learning a new cue for motion in depth Journal of Vision. 4: 288-288. DOI: 10.1167/4.8.288 |
0.541 |
|
2003 |
Backus BT, Matza-Brown D. The contribution of vergence change to the measurement of relative disparity. Journal of Vision. 3: 737-50. PMID 14765957 DOI: 10:1167/3.11.8 |
0.312 |
|
2002 |
Banks MS, Backus BT, Banks RS. Is vertical disparity used to determine azimuth? Vision Research. 42: 801-7. PMID 11927346 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00283-8 |
0.529 |
|
2001 |
Banks MS, Hooge IT, Backus BT. Perceiving slant about a horizontal axis from stereopsis. Journal of Vision. 1: 55-79. PMID 12678602 DOI: 10:1167/1.2.1 |
0.695 |
|
2001 |
Backus BT, Fleet DJ, Parker AJ, Heeger DJ. Human cortical activity correlates with stereoscopic depth perception. Journal of Neurophysiology. 86: 2054-68. PMID 11600661 DOI: 10.1152/Jn.2001.86.4.2054 |
0.524 |
|
2000 |
Ress D, Backus BT, Heeger DJ. Activity in primary visual cortex predicts performance in a visual detection task. Nature Neuroscience. 3: 940-5. PMID 10966626 DOI: 10.1038/78856 |
0.527 |
|
2000 |
Ress D, Backus B, Heeger D. FMRI cortical activity predicts pattern-detection performance Neuroimage. 11: S3. DOI: 10.1016/S1053-8119(00)90938-8 |
0.486 |
|
1999 |
van Ee R, Banks MS, Backus BT. Perceived visual direction near an occluder. Vision Research. 39: 4085-97. PMID 10748941 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00108-X |
0.687 |
|
1999 |
van Ee R, Banks MS, Backus BT. An analysis of binocular slant contrast. Perception. 28: 1121-45. PMID 10694962 |
0.665 |
|
1999 |
Backus BT, Banks MS. Estimator reliability and distance scaling in stereoscopic slant perception. Perception. 28: 217-42. PMID 10615462 |
0.548 |
|
1999 |
Backus BT, Banks MS, van Ee R, Crowell JA. Horizontal and vertical disparity, eye position, and stereoscopic slant perception. Vision Research. 39: 1143-70. PMID 10343832 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00139-4 |
0.688 |
|
1998 |
Banks MS, Backus BT. Extra-retinal and perspective cues cause the small range of the induced effect. Vision Research. 38: 187-94. PMID 9536348 DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00179-X |
0.542 |
|
1997 |
Banks MS, Van Ee R, Backus BT. The computation of binocular visual direction: a re-examination of Mansfield and Legge (1996) Vision Research. 37: 1605-13. PMID 9231227 |
0.667 |
|
1997 |
Banks MS, Backus BT. Conflicts with Extraretinal and Monocular Cues Cause the Small Range of the Induced Effect Perception. 26: 79-79. DOI: 10.1068/v970170 |
0.515 |
|
1996 |
Banks MS, Ehrlich SM, Backus BT, Crowell JA. Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements. Vision Research. 36: 431-43. PMID 8746233 DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00122-0 |
0.609 |
|
1996 |
Backus BT, van Ee R, Banks MS. Horizontal Disparity Pooling Perception. 25: 6-6. DOI: 10.1068/v96p0209 |
0.643 |
|
1996 |
Banks MS, van Ee R, Backus BT. Visual Direction behind and near an Occluder Perception. 25: 7-7. DOI: 10.1068/v96l0908 |
0.658 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |