Year |
Citation |
Score |
2024 |
Prince JS, Alvarez GA, Konkle T. Contrastive learning explains the emergence and function of visual category-selective regions. Science Advances. 10: eadl1776. PMID 39321304 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.adl1776 |
0.784 |
|
2024 |
Park J, Soucy E, Segawa J, Mair R, Konkle T. Immersive scene representation in human visual cortex with ultra-wide-angle neuroimaging. Nature Communications. 15: 5477. PMID 38942766 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-49669-0 |
0.622 |
|
2024 |
Park J, Josephs E, Konkle T. Systematic transition from boundary extension to contraction along an object-to-scene continuum. Journal of Vision. 24: 9. PMID 38252521 DOI: 10.1167/jov.24.1.9 |
0.813 |
|
2023 |
Vinken K, Prince JS, Konkle T, Livingstone MS. The neural code for "face cells" is not face-specific. Science Advances. 9: eadg1736. PMID 37647400 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.adg1736 |
0.69 |
|
2023 |
Kramer LE, Chen YC, Long B, Konkle T, Cohen MR. Contributions of early and mid-level visual cortex to high-level object categorization. Biorxiv : the Preprint Server For Biology. PMID 37398251 DOI: 10.1101/2023.05.31.541514 |
0.713 |
|
2023 |
Doshi FR, Konkle T. Cortical topographic motifs emerge in a self-organized map of object space. Science Advances. 9: eade8187. PMID 37343093 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.ade8187 |
0.811 |
|
2023 |
Park J, Soucy E, Segawa J, Mair R, Konkle T. Ultra-wide angle neuroimaging: insights into immersive scene representation. Biorxiv : the Preprint Server For Biology. PMID 37292806 DOI: 10.1101/2023.05.14.540275 |
0.63 |
|
2023 |
Josephs EL, Hebart MN, Konkle T. Dimensions underlying human understanding of the reachable world. Cognition. 234: 105368. PMID 36641868 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105368 |
0.769 |
|
2022 |
Park J, Josephs E, Konkle T. Ramp-shaped neural tuning supports graded population-level representation of the object-to-scene continuum. Scientific Reports. 12: 18081. PMID 36302932 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-022-21768-2 |
0.836 |
|
2022 |
Janini D, Hamblin C, Deza A, Konkle T. General object-based features account for letter perception. Plos Computational Biology. 18: e1010522. PMID 36155642 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010522 |
0.816 |
|
2022 |
Wang R, Janini D, Konkle T. Mid-level Feature Differences Support Early Animacy and Object Size Distinctions: Evidence from EEG Decoding. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1-11. PMID 35704550 DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_01883 |
0.815 |
|
2022 |
Chen YC, Deza A, Konkle T. How big should this object be? Perceptual influences on viewing-size preferences. Cognition. 225: 105114. PMID 35381479 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105114 |
0.83 |
|
2022 |
Konkle T, Alvarez GA. A self-supervised domain-general learning framework for human ventral stream representation. Nature Communications. 13: 491. PMID 35078981 DOI: 10.1038/s41467-022-28091-4 |
0.581 |
|
2021 |
Tarhan L, De Freitas J, Konkle T. Behavioral and neural representations en route to intuitive action understanding. Neuropsychologia. 163: 108048. PMID 34653497 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.108048 |
0.814 |
|
2021 |
Josephs EL, Zhao H, Konkle T. The world within reach: An image database of reach-relevant environments. Journal of Vision. 21: 14. PMID 34289491 DOI: 10.1167/jov.21.7.14 |
0.767 |
|
2021 |
Magri C, Konkle T, Caramazza A. The contribution of object size, manipulability, and stability on neural responses to inanimate objects. Neuroimage. 237: 118098. PMID 33940141 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118098 |
0.824 |
|
2020 |
Josephs EL, Konkle T. Large-scale dissociations between views of objects, scenes, and reachable-scale environments in visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 117: 29354-29362. PMID 33229533 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1912333117 |
0.81 |
|
2020 |
Tarhan L, Konkle T. Sociality and interaction envelope organize visual action representations. Nature Communications. 11: 3002. PMID 32532982 DOI: 10.1038/S41467-020-16846-W |
0.82 |
|
2020 |
Kallmayer A, Prince J, Konkle T. Comparing representations that support object, scene, and face recognition using representational trajectory analysis. Journal of Vision. 20: 861. DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.11.861 |
0.366 |
|
2020 |
Wang R, Janini D, Kallmayer A, Konkle T. Mid-level feature differences support early EEG-decoding of animacy and object size distinctions Journal of Vision. 20: 738. DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.11.738 |
0.786 |
|
2020 |
Konkle T, Alvarez G. Deepnets do not need category supervision to predict visual system responses to objects Journal of Vision. 20: 498. DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.11.498 |
0.368 |
|
2020 |
Chen Y, Deza A, Konkle T. How big should this object be? Perceptual influences on viewing-size preferences Journal of Vision. 20: 428. DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.11.428 |
0.376 |
|
2020 |
Park J, Josephs E, Konkle T. Neural representation of the visual environment along the continuum from objects to scenes Journal of Vision. 20: 423. DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.11.423 |
0.814 |
|
2020 |
Josephs E, Zhao H, Konkle T. Building the “Reachspace Database”: a large-scale stimulus set of reachable environments Journal of Vision. 20: 193. DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.11.193 |
0.717 |
|
2020 |
Tarhan L, de Freitas J, Alvarez G, Konkle T. Semantic embeddings of verbal descriptions predict action similarity judgments Journal of Vision. 20: 1241. DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.11.1241 |
0.773 |
|
2020 |
Janini D, Konkle T. Approximate number representations emerge in object-trained convolutional neural networks and show human-like signatures of number discrimination Journal of Vision. 20: 1120. DOI: 10.1167/jov.20.11.1120 |
0.772 |
|
2019 |
Tarhan L, Konkle T. Reliability-based voxel selection. Neuroimage. 116350. PMID 31733373 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuroimage.2019.116350 |
0.766 |
|
2019 |
Josephs EL, Konkle T. Perceptual dissociations among views of objects, scenes, and reachable spaces. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 45: 715-728. PMID 31120300 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000626 |
0.829 |
|
2019 |
Janini D, Konkle T. A Pokémon-sized window into the human brain. Nature Human Behaviour. PMID 31061488 DOI: 10.1038/S41562-019-0594-6 |
0.751 |
|
2019 |
Long B, Moher M, Carey S, Konkle T. Real-world size is automatically encoded in preschoolers' object representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. PMID 30985176 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000619 |
0.749 |
|
2019 |
Long B, Moher M, Carey SE, Konkle T. Animacy and object size are reflected in perceptual similarity computations by the preschool years Visual Cognition. 27: 435-451. DOI: 10.17605/Osf.Io/D5Uzg |
0.717 |
|
2019 |
Prince JS, Konkle T. Relating category-selective regions in biological and artificial neural networks Journal of Vision. 19: 60d. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.60D |
0.675 |
|
2019 |
Janini D, Konkle T. Shape features learned for object classification can predict behavioral discrimination of written symbols Journal of Vision. 19: 32d. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.32D |
0.785 |
|
2019 |
Magri C, Long B, Chiou R, Konkle T. Behavioral and Neural Associations between Object Size and Curvature Journal of Vision. 19: 30c. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.30C |
0.81 |
|
2019 |
Tarhan L, Konkle T. Reliability-Based Voxel Selection for Condition-Rich Designs Journal of Vision. 19: 250b. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.250B |
0.747 |
|
2019 |
Josephs EL, Konkle T. Large-scale neural dissociations between views of objects, scenes, and reachable spaces Journal of Vision. 19: 161. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.161 |
0.796 |
|
2019 |
Park J, Josephs E, Konkle T. Reachable or Not? Perceptual judgments of reachability along the object-scene continuum Journal of Vision. 19: 160c. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.160C |
0.815 |
|
2019 |
Maltseva MV, Quinlan DJ, Stubbs KM, Konkle T, Culham JC. Which aspects of size and distance for real objects are coded through the hierarchy of visual areas? Journal of Vision. 19: 15c. DOI: 10.1167/19.10.15C |
0.475 |
|
2018 |
Long B, Yu CP, Konkle T. Mid-level visual features underlie the high-level categorical organization of the ventral stream. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. PMID 30171168 DOI: 10.1073/Pnas.1719616115 |
0.778 |
|
2018 |
Josephs E, Konkle T. Neural representations of reachspaces dissociate from scenes and objects Journal of Vision. 18: 743. DOI: 10.1167/18.10.743 |
0.814 |
|
2018 |
Tarhan L, Konkle T. Predicting the Behavioral Similarity Structure of Visual Actions Journal of Vision. 18: 428. DOI: 10.1167/18.10.428 |
0.787 |
|
2018 |
Magri C, Marantan A, Mahadevan L, Konkle T. Predicting object shape and curvature judgments with a new parameterization of shape Journal of Vision. 18: 422. DOI: 10.1167/18.10.422 |
0.755 |
|
2017 |
Long B, Konkle T. A familiar-size Stroop effect in the absence of basic-level recognition. Cognition. 168: 234-242. PMID 28732302 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2017.06.025 |
0.77 |
|
2017 |
Tarhan L, Konkle T. Low and high level features explain neural response tuning during action observation Journal of Vision. 17: 989. DOI: 10.1167/17.10.989 |
0.78 |
|
2017 |
Long B, Konkle T. Mid-level features are sufficient to drive the animacy and object size organization of the ventral stream Journal of Vision. 17: 575. DOI: 10.1167/17.10.575 |
0.728 |
|
2017 |
Josephs E, Konkle T. Object, scenes, and the spaces in between: Workspaces have distinctive perceptual and semantic content Journal of Vision. 17: 1093. DOI: 10.1167/17.10.1093 |
0.796 |
|
2016 |
Cohen MA, Alvarez GA, Nakayama K, Konkle T. Visual search for object categories is predicted by the representational architecture of high-level visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. jn.00569.2016. PMID 27832600 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00569.2016 |
0.621 |
|
2016 |
Konkle T, Caramazza A. The Large-Scale Organization of Object-Responsive Cortex Is Reflected in Resting-State Network Architecture. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991). PMID 27664960 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhw287 |
0.67 |
|
2016 |
Long B, Konkle T, Cohen MA, Alvarez GA. Mid-level perceptual features distinguish objects of different real-world sizes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 145: 95-109. PMID 26709591 DOI: 10.1037/Xge0000130 |
0.81 |
|
2016 |
Magri C, Konkle T, Caramazza A. Visual object responses of the ventral stream reflect both size and motor-relevance Journal of Vision. 16: 505. DOI: 10.1167/16.12.505 |
0.821 |
|
2016 |
Long B, Carey S, Konkle T. Pre-verbal infants automatically activate real-world object size information Journal of Vision. 16: 1408. DOI: 10.1167/16.12.1408 |
0.7 |
|
2015 |
Cohen M, Nakayama K, Konkle T, Alvarez G. Visual awareness is constrained by the functional organization of the higher-level visual system. Journal of Vision. 15: 585. PMID 26326273 DOI: 10.1167/15.12.585 |
0.625 |
|
2015 |
Konkle T, Wang X, Peelen M, Caramazza A, Bi Y. Convergence and divergence in the neural organization of object responses to pictures and words. Journal of Vision. 15: 375. PMID 26326063 DOI: 10.1167/15.12.375 |
0.827 |
|
2015 |
Long B, Konkle T, Alvarez G. Real-world object size is automatically activated by mid-level shape features. Journal of Vision. 15: 7. PMID 26325695 DOI: 10.1167/15.12.7 |
0.806 |
|
2015 |
Cohen MA, Nakayama K, Konkle T, Stantić M, Alvarez GA. Visual Awareness Is Limited by the Representational Architecture of the Visual System. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1-12. PMID 26226078 DOI: 10.1162/Jocn_A_00855 |
0.632 |
|
2015 |
Park S, Konkle T, Oliva A. Parametric Coding of the Size and Clutter of Natural Scenes in the Human Brain. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991). 25: 1792-805. PMID 24436318 DOI: 10.1093/Cercor/Bht418 |
0.645 |
|
2014 |
Cohen MA, Konkle T, Rhee JY, Nakayama K, Alvarez GA. Processing multiple visual objects is limited by overlap in neural channels. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 111: 8955-60. PMID 24889618 DOI: 10.1073/Pnas.1317860111 |
0.659 |
|
2014 |
Cohen MA, Konkle T, Nakayama K, Alvarez GA. Exploring the representational geometry of object representation in the ventral stream using brain-behavior correlations Journal of Vision. 14: 185-185. DOI: 10.1167/14.10.185 |
0.639 |
|
2014 |
Konkle T, Caramazza A. Object gist features capture the structure of neural responses to objects Journal of Vision. 14: 1292-1292. DOI: 10.1167/14.10.1292 |
0.707 |
|
2013 |
Konkle T, Caramazza A. Tripartite organization of the ventral stream by animacy and object size. The Journal of Neuroscience : the Official Journal of the Society For Neuroscience. 33: 10235-42. PMID 23785139 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0983-13.2013 |
0.721 |
|
2013 |
Brady TF, Konkle T, Gill J, Oliva A, Alvarez GA. Visual long-term memory has the same limit on fidelity as visual working memory. Psychological Science. 24: 981-90. PMID 23630219 DOI: 10.1177/0956797612465439 |
0.738 |
|
2013 |
Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA, Oliva A. Real-world objects are not represented as bound units: independent forgetting of different object details from visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 142: 791-808. PMID 22905874 DOI: 10.1037/a0029649 |
0.785 |
|
2013 |
Long B, Konkle T, Cohen MA, Alvarez GA. Real-world size influences visual search efficiency Journal of Vision. 13: 671-671. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.671 |
0.76 |
|
2013 |
Konkle T, Caramazza A. Large-scale functional distinctions in object cortex are reflected in resting state networks Journal of Vision. 13: 494-494. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.494 |
0.651 |
|
2013 |
Konkle T, Caramazza A. Macro-organization of object responses in occipito-temporal cortex Journal of Vision. 13: 1388-1388. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.1388 |
0.661 |
|
2012 |
Konkle T, Oliva A. A real-world size organization of object responses in occipitotemporal cortex. Neuron. 74: 1114-24. PMID 22726840 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.036 |
0.584 |
|
2012 |
Konkle T, Oliva A. A familiar-size Stroop effect: real-world size is an automatic property of object representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 38: 561-9. PMID 22545601 DOI: 10.1037/A0028294 |
0.642 |
|
2012 |
Rhee J, Konkle T, Brady T, Alvarez G. Does memory enhancement training alter perceptual representations? Journal of Vision. 12: 299-299. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.299 |
0.678 |
|
2012 |
Cohen M, Konkle T, Rhee J, Nakayama K, Alvarez G. High-level neural similarity predicts perceptual competition during encoding of different object categories Journal of Vision. 12: 1269-1269. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.1269 |
0.65 |
|
2012 |
Konkle T, Caramazza A. Comparing Animacy and Real-World Size Object Topography In Occipito-Temporal Cortex: a "Coarse MVPA" approach Journal of Vision. 12: 1109-1109. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.1109 |
0.681 |
|
2011 |
Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA. A review of visual memory capacity: Beyond individual items and toward structured representations. Journal of Vision. 11: 4. PMID 21617025 DOI: 10.1167/11.5.4 |
0.688 |
|
2011 |
Konkle T, Oliva A. Canonical visual size for real-world objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 37: 23-37. PMID 20822298 DOI: 10.1037/A0020413 |
0.642 |
|
2011 |
Konkle T, Oliva A. Organizing visual object knowledge by real-world size in ventral visual cortex Journal of Vision. 11: 883-883. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.883 |
0.607 |
|
2011 |
Park S, Konkle T, Oliva A. Neural coding of the size of space and the amount of clutter in a scene Journal of Vision. 11: 818-818. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.818 |
0.606 |
|
2011 |
Brady T, Konkle T, Alvarez G, Oliva A. Are real-world objects represented as bound units? Independent decay of object details from short-term to long-term memory Journal of Vision. 11: 1289-1289. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.1289 |
0.753 |
|
2011 |
Rhee J, Konkle T, Brady T, Alvarez G. Learning statistical regularities can speed the encoding of information into working memory Journal of Vision. 11: 1279-1279. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.1279 |
0.659 |
|
2011 |
Cohen MA, Nakayama K, Konkle T, Alvarez G. Competition for working memory resources depends on the kind of stimuli being remembered Journal of Vision. 11: 1247-1247. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.1247 |
0.553 |
|
2011 |
Oliva A, Park S, Konkle T. Representing, perceiving, and remembering the shape of visual space Vision in 3d Environments. 308-340. DOI: 10.1017/CBO9780511736261.014 |
0.506 |
|
2010 |
Bedny M, Konkle T, Pelphrey K, Saxe R, Pascual-Leone A. Sensitive period for a multimodal response in human visual motion area MT/MST. Current Biology : Cb. 20: 1900-6. PMID 20970337 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cub.2010.09.044 |
0.367 |
|
2010 |
Konkle T, Brady TF, Alvarez GA, Oliva A. Scene memory is more detailed than you think: the role of categories in visual long-term memory. Psychological Science. 21: 1551-6. PMID 20921574 DOI: 10.1177/0956797610385359 |
0.764 |
|
2010 |
Konkle T, Brady TF, Alvarez GA, Oliva A. Conceptual distinctiveness supports detailed visual long-term memory for real-world objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 139: 558-78. PMID 20677899 DOI: 10.1037/A0019165 |
0.776 |
|
2010 |
Konkle T, Oliva A. Canonical visual sizes for real world objects Journal of Vision. 9: 815-815. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.815 |
0.633 |
|
2010 |
Brady TF, Konkle T, Oliva A. Examining object representation via object memory: Exemplar and state-level object properties are supported by the same underlying features Journal of Vision. 9: 814-814. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.814 |
0.735 |
|
2010 |
Oliva A, Konkle T, Brady TF, Alvarez GA. The high fidelity of scene representation in visual long-term memory Journal of Vision. 9: 586-586. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.586 |
0.752 |
|
2010 |
Alvarez G, Konkle T, Brady T, Gill J, Oliva A. Comparing the fidelity of perception, short-term memory, and long-term memory: Evidence for highly detailed long-term memory representations Journal of Vision. 9: 584-584. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.584 |
0.71 |
|
2010 |
Konkle T, Brady T, Alvarez G, Oliva A. Remembering thousands of objects with high fidelity Journal of Vision. 8: 694-694. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.694 |
0.768 |
|
2010 |
Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA, Oliva A. Compression in visual short-term memory: Using statistical regularities to form more efficient memory representations Journal of Vision. 8: 199-199. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.199 |
0.736 |
|
2010 |
Konkle T, Oliva A. Normative representation of objects and scenes: Evidence from predictable biases in visual perception and memory Journal of Vision. 7: 1049-1049. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.1049 |
0.636 |
|
2010 |
Konkle T, McDaniel E, Greene MR, Oliva A. Constructing depth information in briefly presented scenes Journal of Vision. 6: 466-466. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.466 |
0.665 |
|
2010 |
Oliva A, Konkle T, Greene MR, Torralba A. Not all scene categories are created equal: The role of object and layout diagnosticity in scene gist understanding Journal of Vision. 6: 464-464. DOI: 10.1167/6.6.464 |
0.748 |
|
2010 |
Konkle T, Oliva A. Examining how the real-world size of objects is represented in ventral visual cortex Journal of Vision. 10: 982-982. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.982 |
0.63 |
|
2010 |
Park S, Konkle T, Oliva A. Neural Coding of Scene Volume: the Size of Space Represented across the PPA and LOC Journal of Vision. 10: 1248-1248. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.1248 |
0.605 |
|
2009 |
Konkle T, Moore CI. What can crossmodal aftereffects reveal about neural representation and dynamics? Communicative & Integrative Biology. 2: 479-81. PMID 22811763 DOI: 10.4161/Cib.2.6.9344 |
0.566 |
|
2009 |
Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA. Compression in visual working memory: using statistical regularities to form more efficient memory representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 138: 487-502. PMID 19883132 DOI: 10.1037/A0016797 |
0.705 |
|
2009 |
Brady TF, Konkle T, Oliva A, Alvarez GA. Detecting changes in real-world objects: The relationship between visual long-term memory and change blindness. Communicative & Integrative Biology. 2: 1-3. PMID 19704852 DOI: 10.4161/Cib.2.1.7297 |
0.788 |
|
2009 |
Konkle T, Wang Q, Hayward V, Moore CI. Motion aftereffects transfer between touch and vision. Current Biology : Cb. 19: 745-50. PMID 19361996 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cub.2009.03.035 |
0.518 |
|
2008 |
Brady TF, Konkle T, Alvarez GA, Oliva A. Visual long-term memory has a massive storage capacity for object details. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 105: 14325-9. PMID 18787113 DOI: 10.1073/Pnas.0803390105 |
0.775 |
|
2008 |
Carter O, Konkle T, Wang Q, Hayward V, Moore C. Tactile rivalry demonstrated with an ambiguous apparent-motion quartet. Current Biology : Cb. 18: 1050-4. PMID 18635355 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cub.2008.06.027 |
0.533 |
|
2007 |
Alvarez GA, Konkle T, Oliva A. Searching in dynamic displays: effects of configural predictability and spatiotemporal continuity. Journal of Vision. 7: 12.1-12. PMID 18217807 DOI: 10.1167/7.14.12 |
0.661 |
|
2007 |
Verstynen T, Spencer R, Stinear CM, Konkle T, Diedrichsen J, Byblow WD, Ivry RB. Ipsilateral corticospinal projections do not predict congenital mirror movements: a case report. Neuropsychologia. 45: 844-52. PMID 17023008 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2006.08.019 |
0.486 |
|
2006 |
Verstynen T, Konkle T, Ivry RB. Two types of TMS-induced movement variability after stimulation of the primary motor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 96: 1018-29. PMID 16672298 DOI: 10.1152/Jn.01358.2005 |
0.478 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |