Year |
Citation |
Score |
2025 |
Secora K, Lee B, Coppola M, Smith D. What skills and knowledge should speech-language pathologists have to work with deaf/hard of hearing children who use signed language? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 30: 289-290. PMID 40111191 DOI: 10.1093/jdsade/enae058 |
0.563 |
|
2024 |
Coppola M. Homesign Research, Gesture Studies, and Sign Language Linguistics: The Bigger Picture of Homesign and Homesigners. Topics in Cognitive Science. PMID 39527498 DOI: 10.1111/tops.12766 |
0.626 |
|
2023 |
Walker K, Carrigan E, Coppola M. Early access to language supports number mapping skills in deaf children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 29: 1-18. PMID 38124681 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/enad045 |
0.825 |
|
2023 |
Santos S, Brownell H, Coppola M, Shusterman A, Cordes S. Language experience matters for the emergence of early numerical concepts. Npj Science of Learning. 8: 57. PMID 38071222 DOI: 10.1038/s41539-023-00202-w |
0.594 |
|
2022 |
Kocab A, Senghas A, Coppola M, Snedeker J. Potentially recursive structures emerge quickly when a new language community forms. Cognition. 232: 105261. PMID 36463638 DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105261 |
0.636 |
|
2021 |
Goodwin C, Carrigan E, Walker K, Coppola M. Language not auditory experience is related to parent-reported executive functioning in preschool-aged deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Child Development. PMID 34633656 DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13677 |
0.812 |
|
2020 |
Rissman L, Horton L, Flaherty M, Senghas A, Coppola M, Brentari D, Goldin-Meadow S. The communicative importance of agent-backgrounding: Evidence from homesign and Nicaraguan Sign Language. Cognition. 203: 104332. PMID 32559513 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2020.104332 |
0.82 |
|
2019 |
Gleitman L, Senghas A, Flaherty M, Coppola M, Goldin-Meadow S. The emergence of the formal category "symmetry" in a new sign language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 116: 11705-11711. PMID 31138681 DOI: 10.1073/Pnas.1819872116 |
0.821 |
|
2019 |
Coppola M, Senghas A. Is it language (yet)? The allure of the gesture-language binary. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 40: e50. PMID 29342513 DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X1500285X |
0.837 |
|
2019 |
Abner N, Flaherty M, Stangl K, Coppola M, Brentari D, Goldin-Meadow S. The noun-verb distinction in established and emergent sign systems: Supplemental Material Language. 95. DOI: 10.1353/Lan.2019.0050 |
0.758 |
|
2019 |
Abner N, Flaherty M, Stangl K, Coppola M, Brentari D, Goldin-Meadow S. The noun-verb distinction in established and emergent sign systems Language. 95: 230-267. DOI: 10.1353/Lan.2019.0030 |
0.773 |
|
2017 |
Brentari D, Coppola M, Cho PW, Senghas A. Handshape complexity as a precursor to phonology: Variation, Emergence, and Acquisition. Language Acquisition. 24: 283-306. PMID 33033424 DOI: 10.1080/10489223.2016.1187614 |
0.769 |
|
2017 |
Gagne DL, Coppola M. Visible Social Interactions Do Not Support the Development of False Belief Understanding in the Absence of Linguistic Input: Evidence from Deaf Adult Homesigners. Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 837. PMID 28626432 DOI: 10.3389/Fpsyg.2017.00837 |
0.784 |
|
2017 |
Carrigan EM, Coppola M. Successful communication does not drive language development: Evidence from adult homesign Cognition. 158: 10-27. PMID 27771538 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2016.09.012 |
0.83 |
|
2017 |
Jenkins T, Coppola M, Coelho C. Effects of gesture restriction on quality of narrative production Gesture. 16: 416-431. DOI: 10.1075/Gest.00003.Jen |
0.437 |
|
2015 |
Horton L, Goldin-Meadow S, Coppola M, Senghas A, Brentari D. Forging a morphological system out of two dimensions: Agentivity and number. Open Linguistics. 1: 596-613. PMID 26740937 DOI: 10.1515/Opli-2015-0021 |
0.841 |
|
2015 |
Goldin-Meadow S, Brentari D, Coppola M, Horton L, Senghas A. Watching language grow in the manual modality: nominals, predicates, and handshapes. Cognition. 136: 381-95. PMID 25546342 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cognition.2014.11.029 |
0.858 |
|
2014 |
Applebaum L, Coppola M, Goldin-Meadow S. Prosody in a communication system developed without a language model. Sign Language and Linguistics. 17: 181-212. PMID 25574153 DOI: 10.1075/Sll.17.2.02App |
0.821 |
|
2014 |
Coppola M, Brentari D. From iconic handshapes to grammatical contrasts: longitudinal evidence from a child homesigner. Frontiers in Psychology. 5: 830. PMID 25191283 DOI: 10.3389/Fpsyg.2014.00830 |
0.566 |
|
2014 |
Richie R, Yang C, Coppola M. Modeling the emergence of lexicons in homesign systems. Topics in Cognitive Science. 6: 183-95. PMID 24482343 DOI: 10.1111/Tops.12076 |
0.846 |
|
2013 |
Brentari D, Coppola M. What sign language creation teaches us about language. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews. Cognitive Science. 4: 201-11. PMID 26304196 DOI: 10.1002/Wcs.1212 |
0.68 |
|
2013 |
Spaepen E, Coppola M, Flaherty M, Spelke E, Goldin-Meadow S. Generating a lexicon without a language model: Do words for number count? Journal of Memory and Language. 69. PMID 24187432 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jml.2013.05.004 |
0.781 |
|
2013 |
Coppola M, Spaepen E, Goldin-Meadow S. Communicating about quantity without a language model: number devices in homesign grammar. Cognitive Psychology. 67: 1-25. PMID 23872365 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogpsych.2013.05.003 |
0.832 |
|
2013 |
Brentari D, Coppola M, Jung A, Goldin-Meadow S. Acquiring word class distinctions in American Sign Language: Evidence from handshape. Language Learning and Development : the Official Journal of the Society For Language Development. 9: 130-150. PMID 23671406 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2012.679540 |
0.736 |
|
2012 |
Brentari D, Coppola M, Mazzoni L, Goldin-Meadow S. When does a system become phonological? Handshape production in gesturers, signers, and homesigners. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory. 30: 1-31. PMID 23723534 DOI: 10.1007/S11049-011-9145-1 |
0.798 |
|
2011 |
Spaepen E, Coppola M, Spelke ES, Carey SE, Goldin-Meadow S. Number without a language model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108: 3163-8. PMID 21300893 DOI: 10.1073/Pnas.1015975108 |
0.838 |
|
2005 |
So WC, Coppola M, Licciardello V, Goldin-Meadow S. The seeds of spatial grammar in the manual modality. Cognitive Science. 29: 1029-43. PMID 21702801 DOI: 10.1207/S15516709Cog0000_38 |
0.796 |
|
2005 |
Coppola M, Newport EL. Grammatical Subjects in home sign: Abstract linguistic structure in adult primary gesture systems without linguistic input. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102: 19249-53. PMID 16357199 DOI: 10.1073/Pnas.0509306102 |
0.824 |
|
2001 |
Senghas A, Coppola M. Children creating language: how Nicaraguan sign language acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science. 12: 323-8. PMID 11476100 DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00359 |
0.836 |
|
1997 |
Ullman MT, Corkin S, Coppola M, Hickok G, Growdon JH, Koroshetz WJ, Pinker S. A Neural Dissociation within Language: Evidence that the Mental Dictionary Is Part of Declarative Memory, and that Grammatical Rules Are Processed by the Procedural System. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 9: 266-76. PMID 23962016 DOI: 10.1162/Jocn.1997.9.2.266 |
0.43 |
|
1994 |
Kim JJ, Marcus GF, Pinker S, Hollander M, Coppola M. Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure. Journal of Child Language. 21: 173-209. PMID 8006091 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000900008710 |
0.494 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |