Year |
Citation |
Score |
2020 |
Parker S, Heathcote A, Finkbeiner M. Using evidence accumulation modeling to quantify the relative contributions of spatial attention and saccade preparation in perceptual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 46: 416-433. PMID 32223293 DOI: 10.1037/Xhp0000723 |
0.364 |
|
2016 |
Tillman G, Eidels A, Finkbeiner M. A reach-to-touch investigation on the nature of reading in the Stroop task. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 27549606 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-016-1190-8 |
0.415 |
|
2015 |
Finkbeiner M, Heathcote A. Distinguishing the time- and magnitude-difference accounts of the Simon effect: Evidence from the reach-to-touch paradigm. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 26715514 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-015-1044-9 |
0.356 |
|
2015 |
Quek GL, Finkbeiner M. The upper-hemifield advantage for masked face processing: Not just an attentional bias. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. PMID 26515816 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-015-0965-7 |
0.707 |
|
2015 |
Ocampo B, Al-Janabi S, Finkbeiner M. Direct evidence of cognitive control without perceptual awareness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 22: 1083-8. PMID 25404552 DOI: 10.3758/S13423-014-0766-3 |
0.748 |
|
2015 |
Khalid S, Ansorge U, Finkbeiner M. Supraliminal but No Subliminal Priming by High-Spatial Frequency Faces in a Face-Sex Discrimination Task Psychology. 6: 1486-1509. DOI: 10.4236/Psych.2015.612146 |
0.441 |
|
2014 |
Quek GL, Finkbeiner M. Gaining the upper hand: evidence of vertical asymmetry in sex-categorisation of human hands. Advances in Cognitive Psychology / University of Finance and Management in Warsaw. 10: 131-43. PMID 25674193 DOI: 10.5709/Acp-0164-8 |
0.678 |
|
2014 |
Quek GL, Finkbeiner M. Face-sex categorization is better above fixation than below: Evidence from the reach-to-touch paradigm. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 14: 1407-19. PMID 24763922 DOI: 10.3758/S13415-014-0282-Y |
0.724 |
|
2014 |
Al-Janabi S, Finkbeiner M. Responding to the direction of the eyes: in search of the masked gaze-cueing effect. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 76: 148-61. PMID 24132711 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-013-0570-6 |
0.76 |
|
2014 |
Finkbeiner M, Coltheart M, Coltheart V. Pointing the way to new constraints on the dynamical claims of computational models. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 40: 172-85. PMID 23750962 DOI: 10.1037/A0033169 |
0.367 |
|
2014 |
Quek GL, Finkbeiner M. Face-perception is superior in the upper visual field: Evidence from masked priming Visual Cognition. 22: 1038-1042. DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2014.960732 |
0.678 |
|
2013 |
Ocampo B, Finkbeiner M. The negative compatibility effect with relevant masks: a case for automatic motor inhibition. Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 822. PMID 24265623 DOI: 10.3389/Fpsyg.2013.00822 |
0.324 |
|
2013 |
Langdon R, Finkbeiner M, Connors MH, Connaughton E. Masked and unmasked priming in schizophrenia. Consciousness and Cognition. 22: 1206-13. PMID 24021849 DOI: 10.1016/J.Concog.2013.07.009 |
0.345 |
|
2013 |
Quek GL, Finkbeiner M. Spatial and temporal attention modulate the early stages of face processing: behavioural evidence from a reaching paradigm. Plos One. 8: e57365. PMID 23468977 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0057365 |
0.712 |
|
2013 |
Khalid S, Finkbeiner M, König P, Ansorge U. Subcortical human face processing? Evidence from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 39: 989-1002. PMID 23244041 DOI: 10.1037/A0030867 |
0.382 |
|
2013 |
Friedman J, Brown S, Finkbeiner M. Linking cognitive and reaching trajectories via intermittent movement control Journal of Mathematical Psychology. 57: 140-151. DOI: 10.1016/J.Jmp.2013.06.005 |
0.494 |
|
2012 |
Al-Janabi S, Finkbeiner M. Effective processing of masked eye gaze requires volitional control. Experimental Brain Research. 216: 433-43. PMID 22101495 DOI: 10.1177/1550059412444821 |
0.754 |
|
2012 |
Quek G, Finkbeiner M. Processing faces on the vertical meridian: evidence for above-fixation superiority F1000research. 3. DOI: 10.7490/F1000Research.1092694.1 |
0.691 |
|
2012 |
Finkbeiner M, Friedman J, Quek G, Al-Janabi S. Using reaching trajectories to reveal the dynamics of stimulus categorisation Clinical Eeg and Neuroscience. 43. DOI: 10.1177/1550059412444821 |
0.68 |
|
2011 |
Finkbeiner M, Friedman J. The flexibility of nonconsciously deployed cognitive processes: evidence from masked congruence priming. Plos One. 6: e17095. PMID 21347336 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0017095 |
0.586 |
|
2011 |
Zopf R, Truong S, Finkbeiner M, Friedman J, Williams MA. Viewing and feeling touch modulates hand position for reaching. Neuropsychologia. 49: 1287-93. PMID 21320514 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2011.02.012 |
0.602 |
|
2011 |
Finkbeiner M. Subliminal priming with nearly perfect performance in the prime-classification task. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics. 73: 1255-65. PMID 21279497 DOI: 10.3758/S13414-011-0088-8 |
0.391 |
|
2010 |
Janssen N, Melinger A, Mahon BZ, Finkbeiner M, Caramazza A. The word class effect in the picture-word interference paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 63: 1233-46. PMID 19998070 DOI: 10.1080/17470210903377380 |
0.736 |
|
2010 |
Mousikou P, Coltheart M, Finkbeiner M, Saunders S. Can the dual-route cascaded computational model of reading offer a valid account of the masked onset priming effect? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 63: 984-1003. PMID 19742386 DOI: 10.1080/17470210903156586 |
0.383 |
|
2010 |
Truong S, Zopf R, Finkbeiner M, Friedman J, Williams M. Perceptual Body Illusion Affects Action Journal of Vision. 10: 1052-1052. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.1052 |
0.538 |
|
2009 |
Finkbeiner M, Palermo R. The role of spatial attention in nonconscious processing: a comparison of face and nonface stimuli. Psychological Science. 20: 42-51. PMID 19152540 DOI: 10.1111/J.1467-9280.2008.02256.X |
0.388 |
|
2008 |
Finkbeiner M, Caramazza A. Modulating the masked congruence priming effect with the hands and the mouth. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 34: 894-918. PMID 18665734 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.894 |
0.578 |
|
2008 |
Knobel M, Finkbeiner M, Caramazza A. The many places of frequency: evidence for a novel locus of the lexical frequency effect in word production. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 25: 256-86. PMID 18568814 DOI: 10.1080/02643290701502425 |
0.748 |
|
2008 |
Hartsuiker RJ, Costa A, Finkbeiner M. Bilingualism: functional and neural perspectives. Acta Psychologica. 128: 413-5. PMID 18565484 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2008.05.001 |
0.495 |
|
2008 |
Finkbeiner M, Forster KI. Attention, intention and domain-specific processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 12: 59-64. PMID 18178512 DOI: 10.1016/J.Tics.2007.11.003 |
0.605 |
|
2008 |
Finkbeiner M, Song JH, Nakayama K, Caramazza A. Engaging the motor system with masked orthographic primes: A kinematic analysis Visual Cognition. 16: 11-22. DOI: 10.1080/13506280701203838 |
0.574 |
|
2007 |
Almeida J, Knobel M, Finkbeiner M, Caramazza A. The locus of the frequency effect in picture naming: when recognizing is not enough. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 14: 1177-82. PMID 18229493 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193109 |
0.746 |
|
2007 |
Finkbeiner M, Slotnick SD, Moo LR, Caramazza A. Involuntary capture of attention produces domain-specific activation. Neuroreport. 18: 975-9. PMID 17558280 DOI: 10.1097/Wnr.0B013E3281668Bcc |
0.548 |
|
2007 |
Nakayama K, Song J, Finkbeiner M, Caramazza A. Hand trajectories reveal cognitive states Journal of Vision. 7: 568-568. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.568 |
0.448 |
|
2006 |
Finkbeiner M, Almeida J, Caramazza A. Letter identification processes in reading: Distractor interference reveals an automatically engaged, domain-specific mechanism. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 23: 1083-103. PMID 21049369 DOI: 10.1080/02643290600665778 |
0.64 |
|
2006 |
Finkbeiner M, Caramazza A. Now you see it, now you don't: on turning semantic interference into facilitation in a Stroop-like task. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 42: 790-6. PMID 17131581 DOI: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70419-2 |
0.569 |
|
2006 |
Finkbeiner M, Almeida J, Janssen N, Caramazza A. Lexical selection in bilingual speech production does not involve language suppression. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 32: 1075-89. PMID 16938047 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1075 |
0.681 |
|
2006 |
Finkbeiner M, Gollan TH, Caramazza A. Lexical access in bilingual speakers: What's the (hard) problem? Bilingualism. 9: 153-166. DOI: 10.1017/S1366728906002501 |
0.644 |
|
2006 |
Finkbeiner M, Caramazza A. Lexical selection is not a competitive process: A reply to La Heij et al. (2006) Cortex. 42: 1032-1036. DOI: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70210-7 |
0.455 |
|
2004 |
Finkbeiner M, Forster K, Nicol J, Nakamura K. The role of polysemy in masked semantic and translation priming Journal of Memory and Language. 51: 1-22. DOI: 10.1016/J.Jml.2004.01.004 |
0.699 |
|
2003 |
Finkbeiner M, Nicol J. Semantic category effects in second language word learning Applied Psycholinguistics. 24: 369-383. DOI: 10.1017/S0142716403000195 |
0.562 |
|
2002 |
Finkbeiner M, Nicol J, Greth D, Nakamura K. The role of language in memory for actions. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 31: 447-57. PMID 12528427 DOI: 10.1023/A:1021204802485 |
0.585 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |