Year |
Citation |
Score |
2021 |
Vandierendonck A. How the Working Memory with Distributed Executive Control Model Accounts for Task Switching and Dual-Task Coordination Costs. Journal of Cognition. 4: 2. PMID 33506168 DOI: 10.5334/joc.138 |
0.454 |
|
2018 |
Vandierendonck A. Working memory benchmarks-A missed opportunity: Comment on Oberauer et al. (2018). Psychological Bulletin. 144: 963-971. PMID 30148381 DOI: 10.1037/Bul0000159 |
0.32 |
|
2017 |
Vandierendonck A, Loncke M, Hartsuiker RJ, Desmet T. The role of executive control in resolving grammatical number conflict in sentence comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 1-61. PMID 28056625 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1276610 |
0.399 |
|
2016 |
Vandierendonck A. A Working Memory System With Distributed Executive Control. Perspectives On Psychological Science : a Journal of the Association For Psychological Science. 11: 74-100. PMID 26817727 DOI: 10.1177/1745691615596790 |
0.439 |
|
2015 |
Vandierendonck A. Modality independence of order coding in working memory: Evidence from cross-modal order interference at recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 1-19. PMID 25801664 DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1032987 |
0.518 |
|
2014 |
Vandierendonck A. Symbiosis of executive and selective attention in working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 588. PMID 25152723 DOI: 10.3389/Fnhum.2014.00588 |
0.517 |
|
2013 |
Vandierendonck A. On the role of dissipating inhibition in task switching Zeitschrift Fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology. 221: 61-62. DOI: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000131 |
0.375 |
|
2012 |
Vandierendonck A, Demanet J, Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F. A chain-retrieval model for voluntary task switching. Cognitive Psychology. 65: 241-83. PMID 22609806 DOI: 10.1016/J.Cogpsych.2012.04.003 |
0.681 |
|
2012 |
Vandierendonck A. Role of working memory in task switching Psychologica Belgica. 52: 229-253. DOI: 10.5334/Pb-52-2-3-229 |
0.518 |
|
2011 |
Szmalec A, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A, Kemps E. Control of interference during working memory updating. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 37: 137-51. PMID 20731517 DOI: 10.1037/A0020365 |
0.608 |
|
2011 |
Loncke M, Desmet T, Vandierendonck A, Hartsuiker RJ. Executive control is shared between sentence processing and digit maintenance: Evidence from a strictly timed dual-task paradigm Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 23: 886-911. DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2011.586625 |
0.438 |
|
2010 |
Vermeiren A, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Switch performance in peripherally and centrally triggered saccades. Experimental Brain Research. 206: 243-8. PMID 20809243 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-010-2401-5 |
0.334 |
|
2010 |
Van Loy B, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Cognitive control in cued task switching with transition cues: cue processing, task processing, and cue-task transition congruency. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 63: 1916-35. PMID 20574933 DOI: 10.1080/17470211003779160 |
0.475 |
|
2010 |
Vandierendonck A, Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F. Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin. 136: 601-26. PMID 20565170 DOI: 10.1037/A0019791 |
0.689 |
|
2010 |
Demanet J, Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Voluntary task switching under load: contribution of top-down and bottom-up factors in goal-directed behavior. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 17: 387-93. PMID 20551363 DOI: 10.3758/Pbr.17.3.387 |
0.699 |
|
2010 |
Liefooghe B, Demanet J, Vandierendonck A. Persisting activation in voluntary task switching: it all depends on the instructions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 17: 381-6. PMID 20551362 DOI: 10.3758/Pbr.17.3.381 |
0.487 |
|
2010 |
Vandamme K, Szmalec A, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Are voluntary switches corrected repetitions? Psychophysiology. 47: 1176-81. PMID 20456659 DOI: 10.1111/J.1469-8986.2010.01032.X |
0.515 |
|
2010 |
Vandierendonck A. The role of executive control in task switching Cognition and Neuropsychology: International Perspectives On Psychological Science. 35-47. DOI: 10.4324/9780203845820 |
0.375 |
|
2010 |
Imbo I, Vandierendonck A. Instruction and load effects on high-skill and low-skill individuals: A study in the domain of mental arithmetic European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 22: 964-989. DOI: 10.1080/09541440903150196 |
0.357 |
|
2009 |
Liefooghe B, Demanet J, Vandierendonck A. Is advance reconfiguration in voluntary task switching affected by the design employed? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 62: 850-7. PMID 19132632 DOI: 10.1080/17470210802570994 |
0.49 |
|
2009 |
Szmalec A, Duyck W, Vandierendonck A, Mata AB, Page MP. The Hebb repetition effect as a laboratory analogue of novel word learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 62: 435-43. PMID 18785073 DOI: 10.1080/17470210802386375 |
0.369 |
|
2009 |
Depoorter A, Vandierendonck A. Evidence for modality-independent order coding in working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 62: 531-49. PMID 18609385 DOI: 10.1080/17470210801995002 |
0.497 |
|
2009 |
Szmalec A, Demanet J, Vandierendonck A, Verbruggen F. Investigating the role of conflict resolution in memory updating by means of the one-back choice RT task. Psychological Research. 73: 390-406. PMID 18437414 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-008-0149-3 |
0.714 |
|
2009 |
Imbo I, Szmalec A, Vandierendonck A. The role of structure in age-related increases in visuo-spatial working memory span Psychologica Belgica. 49: 275-291. DOI: 10.5334/Pb-49-4-275 |
0.403 |
|
2008 |
Vandierendonck A, Christiaens E, Liefooghe B. On the representation of task information in task switching: evidence from task and dimension switching. Memory & Cognition. 36: 1248-61. PMID 18927041 DOI: 10.3758/Mc.36.7.1248 |
0.528 |
|
2008 |
Liefooghe B, Barrouillet P, Vandierendonck A, Camos V. Working memory costs of task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 34: 478-94. PMID 18444750 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.478 |
0.518 |
|
2008 |
Verbruggen F, Logan GD, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Short-term aftereffects of response inhibition: repetition priming or between-trial control adjustments? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance. 34: 413-26. PMID 18377179 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.413 |
0.667 |
|
2008 |
Szmalec A, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A, De Baene W, Verguts T, Notebaert W. Stimulus ambiguity elicits response conflict. Neuroscience Letters. 435: 158-62. PMID 18329807 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neulet.2008.02.023 |
0.688 |
|
2008 |
Imbo I, Vandierendonck A. Practice effects on strategy selection and strategy efficiency in simple mental arithmetic. Psychological Research. 72: 528-41. PMID 17906877 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-007-0128-0 |
0.313 |
|
2008 |
Imbo I, Vandierendonck A. Effects of problem size, operation, and working-memory span on simple-arithmetic strategies: differences between children and adults? Psychological Research. 72: 331-46. PMID 17457605 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-007-0112-8 |
0.312 |
|
2008 |
Vandierendonck A, Deschuyteneer M, Depoorter A, Drieghe D. Input monitoring and response selection as components of executive control in pro-saccades and anti-saccades. Psychological Research. 72: 1-11. PMID 16924540 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-006-0078-Y |
0.46 |
|
2008 |
Duverne S, Lemaire P, Vandierendonck A. Do working-memory executive components mediate the effects of age on strategy selection or on strategy execution? Insights from arithmetic problem solving. Psychological Research. 72: 27-38. PMID 16838186 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-006-0071-5 |
0.416 |
|
2007 |
Imbo I, Vandierendonck A. Do multiplication and division strategies rely on executive and phonological working memory resources? Memory & Cognition. 35: 1759-71. PMID 18062552 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193508 |
0.389 |
|
2007 |
Imbo I, Vandierendonck A, Rosseel Y. The influence of problem features and individual differences on strategic performance in simple arithmetic. Memory & Cognition. 35: 454-63. PMID 17691144 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193285 |
0.315 |
|
2007 |
Szmalec A, Vandierendonck A. Estimating the executive demands of a one-back choice reaction time task by means of the selective interference paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 60: 1116-39. PMID 17654395 DOI: 10.1080/17470210600896484 |
0.537 |
|
2007 |
Imbo I, Vandierendonck A, De Rammelaere S. The role of working memory in the carry operation of mental arithmetic: number and value of the carry. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 60: 708-31. PMID 17455078 DOI: 10.1080/17470210600762447 |
0.333 |
|
2007 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A, Demanet J. Short cue presentations encourage advance task preparation: a recipe to diminish the residual switch cost. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 33: 342-56. PMID 17352616 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.342 |
0.662 |
|
2007 |
Imbo I, Vandierendonck A. The development of strategy use in elementary school children: working memory and individual differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 96: 284-309. PMID 17046017 DOI: 10.1016/J.Jecp.2006.09.001 |
0.382 |
|
2007 |
Imbo I, Vandierendonck A, Vergauwe E. The role of working memory in carrying and borrowing. Psychological Research. 71: 467-83. PMID 16622702 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-006-0044-8 |
0.328 |
|
2007 |
Demanet J, Dhont K, Notebaert L, Pattyn S, Vandierendonck A. Pixelating familiar people in the media: Should masking be taken at face value? Psychologica Belgica. 47: 261-276. DOI: 10.5334/Pb-47-4-261 |
0.306 |
|
2007 |
Imbo I, Vandierendonck A. The role of phonological and executive working memory resources in simple arithmetic strategies European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 19: 910-933. DOI: 10.1080/09541440601051571 |
0.383 |
|
2007 |
Liefooghe B, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A, Fias W, Gevers W. Task switching and across-trial distance priming are independent European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 19: 1-16. DOI: 10.1080/09541440500492033 |
0.66 |
|
2006 |
Stevens M, Lammertyn J, Verbruggen F, Vandierendonck A. Tscope: A C library for programming cognitive experiments on the MS windows platform. Behavior Research Methods. 38: 280-6. PMID 16956104 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03192779 |
0.516 |
|
2006 |
Deschuyteneer M, Vandierendonck A, Muyllaert I. Does solution of mental arithmetic problems such as 2 + 6 and 3 x 8 rely on the process of "Memory updating"? Experimental Psychology. 53: 198-208. PMID 16955729 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.3.198 |
0.477 |
|
2006 |
Verbruggen F, Notebaert W, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Stimulus- and response-conflict-induced cognitive control in the flanker task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 13: 328-33. PMID 16893003 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193852 |
0.643 |
|
2006 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. Selective stopping in task switching: The role of response selection and response execution. Experimental Psychology. 53: 48-57. PMID 16610272 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.48 |
0.642 |
|
2006 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. The effect of interference in the early processing stages on response inhibition in the stop signal task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006). 59: 190-203. PMID 16556567 DOI: 10.1080/17470210500151386 |
0.649 |
|
2006 |
Franssen V, Vandierendonck A, Van Hiel A. Duration estimation and the phonological loop: articulatory suppression and irrelevant sounds. Psychological Research. 70: 304-16. PMID 16001277 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-005-0217-X |
0.401 |
|
2006 |
Vandierendonck A, Dierckx V, Van der Beken HV. Interaction of knowledge and working memory in reasoning about relations Advances in Psychology. 138: 53-84. DOI: 10.1016/S0166-4115(06)80027-0 |
0.342 |
|
2005 |
Deschuyteneer M, Vandierendonck A. The role of response selection and input monitoring in solving simple arithmetical products. Memory & Cognition. 33: 1472-83. PMID 16615394 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193379 |
0.458 |
|
2005 |
Szmalec A, Vandierendonck A, Kemps E. Response selection involves executive control: evidence from the selective interference paradigm. Memory & Cognition. 33: 531-41. PMID 16156187 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03193069 |
0.542 |
|
2005 |
Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A, Muyllaert I, Verbruggen F, Vanneste W. The phonological loop in task alternation and task repetition. Memory (Hove, England). 13: 550-60. PMID 16020382 DOI: 10.1080/09658210444000250 |
0.704 |
|
2005 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Notebaert W, Vandierendonck A. Effects of stimulus-stimulus compatibility and stimulus-response compatibility on response inhibition. Acta Psychologica. 120: 307-26. PMID 15993830 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2005.05.003 |
0.677 |
|
2005 |
Goethals I, Audenaert K, Jacobs F, Van de Wiele C, Ham H, Pyck H, Vandierendonck A, Van Heeringen C, Dierckx R. Blunted prefrontal perfusion in depressed patients performing the Tower of London task. Psychiatry Research. 139: 31-40. PMID 15927455 DOI: 10.1016/J.Pscychresns.2004.09.007 |
0.359 |
|
2005 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Szmalec A, Vandierendonck A. Inhibiting responses when switching: Does it matter? Experimental Psychology. 52: 125-30. PMID 15850159 DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169.52.2.125 |
0.661 |
|
2005 |
Kemps E, Szmalec A, Vandierendonck A, Crevits L. Visuo-spatial processing in Parkinson's disease: evidence for diminished visuo-spatial sketch pad and central executive resources. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders. 11: 181-6. PMID 15823483 DOI: 10.1016/J.Parkreldis.2004.10.010 |
0.435 |
|
2005 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. On the difference between response inhibition and negative priming: evidence from simple and selective stopping. Psychological Research. 69: 262-71. PMID 15750869 DOI: 10.1007/S00426-004-0177-6 |
0.621 |
|
2005 |
Imbo I, De Rammelaere S, Vandierendonck A. New insights in the role of working memory in carry and borrow operations Psychologica Belgica. 45: 101-121. DOI: 10.5334/Pb-45-2-101 |
0.315 |
|
2005 |
Deschuyteneer M, Vandierendonck A. Are "input monitoring" and "response selection" involved in solving simple mental arithmetical sums? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 17: 347-370. DOI: 10.1080/09541440440000032 |
0.465 |
|
2004 |
Goethals I, Audenaert K, Jacobs F, Van de Wiele C, Pyck H, Ham H, Vandierendonck A, van Heeringen C, Dierckx R. Application of a neuropsychological activation probe with SPECT: the 'Tower of London' task in healthy volunteers. Nuclear Medicine Communications. 25: 177-82. PMID 15154709 DOI: 10.1097/00006231-200402000-00015 |
0.488 |
|
2004 |
Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. The interaction between stop signal inhibition and distractor interference in the flanker and Stroop task. Acta Psychologica. 116: 21-37. PMID 15111228 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2003.12.011 |
0.695 |
|
2004 |
Vandierendonck A, Kemps E, Fastame MC, Szmalec A. Working memory components of the Corsi blocks task. British Journal of Psychology (London, England : 1953). 95: 57-79. PMID 15005868 DOI: 10.1348/000712604322779460 |
0.512 |
|
2004 |
Crevits L, Vandierendonck A, Stuyven E, Verschaete S, Wildenbeest J. Effect of intention and visual fixation disengagement on prosaccades in Parkinson's disease patients. Neuropsychologia. 42: 624-32. PMID 14725800 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2003.10.005 |
0.341 |
|
2004 |
Vandorpe S, De Rammelaere S, Vandierendonck A. The odd-even effect in multiplication: Familiarity with even numbers or a parity rule after all? Psychologica Belgica. 44: 143-157. DOI: 10.5334/Pb.1021 |
0.34 |
|
2003 |
De Rammelaere S, Vandierendonck A. Number comparison under executive dual-task Psychologica Belgica. 43: 259-268. DOI: 10.5334/Pb.1012 |
0.5 |
|
2003 |
Vandierendonck A, Szmalec A. An Asymmetry in the Visuo-Spatial Demands of Forward and Backward Recall in the Corsi Blocks Task Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 23: 225-231. DOI: 10.2190/Meeh-Uyhg-Uy9J-Mq04 |
0.523 |
|
2003 |
Duyck W, Vandierendonck A, De Vooght G. Conditional reasoning with a spatial content requires visuo-spatial working memory Thinking and Reasoning. 9: 267-287. DOI: 10.1080/1354678034000259 |
0.459 |
|
2003 |
Duyck W, Szmalec A, Kemps E, Vandierendonck A. Verbal working memory is involved in associative word learning unless visual codes are available Journal of Memory and Language. 48: 527-541. DOI: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00533-8 |
0.362 |
|
2002 |
Goethals I, Audenaert K, Jacobs F, Van De Wiele C, Vermeir G, Vandierendonck A, Van Heeringen C, Dierckx R. Toward clinical application of neuropsychological activation probes with SPECT: a spatial working memory task. Journal of Nuclear Medicine : Official Publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine. 43: 1426-31. PMID 12411543 |
0.396 |
|
2002 |
Caessens B, Vandierendonck A. Inhibition and blindness to response-compatible stimuli: a reappraisal. Acta Psychologica. 111: 45-57. PMID 12102120 DOI: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00018-5 |
0.457 |
|
2002 |
Franssen V, Vandierendonck A. Time estimation: does the reference memory mediate the effect of knowledge of results? Acta Psychologica. 109: 239-67. PMID 11881902 DOI: 10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00059-2 |
0.456 |
|
2001 |
De Rammelaere S, Stuyven E, Vandierendonck A. Verifying simple arithmetic sums and products: are the phonological loop and the central executive involved? Memory & Cognition. 29: 267-73. PMID 11352209 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03194920 |
0.356 |
|
2000 |
Vandierendonck A. Analyzing human random time generation behavior: a methodology and a computer program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers : a Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc. 32: 555-65. PMID 11189856 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03200828 |
0.48 |
|
2000 |
Vandierendonck A. Is judgment of random time intervals biased and capacity-limited? Psychological Research. 63: 199-209. PMID 10946594 DOI: 10.1007/Pl00008179 |
0.502 |
|
2000 |
Stuyven E, Van der Goten K, Vandierendonck A, Claeys K, Crevits L. The effect of cognitive load on saccadic eye movements. Acta Psychologica. 104: 69-85. PMID 10769940 DOI: 10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00054-2 |
0.504 |
|
2000 |
Vandierendonck A. Executive functions and task switching Psychologica Belgica. 40: 211-226. DOI: 10.5334/Pb.963 |
0.473 |
|
2000 |
Allan LG, Keyser VD, d'Ydewalle G, Vandierendonck A. Time and the Dynamic Control of Behavior The American Journal of Psychology. 113: 455. DOI: 10.2307/1423368 |
0.301 |
|
1999 |
Martein R, Kemps E, Vandierendonck A. The role of working memory in a double span task Psychologica Belgica. 39: 15-27. DOI: 10.5334/Pb.939 |
0.456 |
|
1999 |
De Rammelaere S, Stuyven E, Vandierendonck A. The contribution of working memory resources in the verification of simple mental arithmetic sums Psychological Research. 62: 72-77. DOI: 10.1007/S004260050041 |
0.47 |
|
1998 |
Claeys K, Crevits L, Stuyven E, van der Goten K, Depuydt C, Vandierendonck A. Parallel visual and memory processes. Documenta Ophthalmologica. Advances in Ophthalmology. 95: 349-58. PMID 10532415 DOI: 10.1023/A:1001816709627 |
0.528 |
|
1998 |
Vandierendonck A, De Vooght G, Van der Goten K. Interfering with the central executive by means of a random interval repetition task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. a, Human Experimental Psychology. 51: 197-218. PMID 9532967 DOI: 10.1080/713755748 |
0.542 |
|
1997 |
Vandierendonck A, De Vooght G. Working memory constraints on linear reasoning with spatial and temporal contents. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. a, Human Experimental Psychology. 50: 803-20. PMID 9450380 DOI: 10.1080/713755735 |
0.364 |
|
1996 |
Vandierendonck A. Evidence for Mental-model-based Reasoning: A Comparison of Reasoning with Time and Space Concepts Thinking & Reasoning. 2: 249-272. DOI: 10.1080/135467896394438 |
0.375 |
|
1995 |
Vandierendonck A. A parallel rule activation and rule synthesis model for generalization in category learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2: 442-59. PMID 24203784 DOI: 10.3758/Bf03210982 |
0.312 |
|
1988 |
Vandierendonck A, Van Hoe R, De Soete G. Menu search as a function of menu organization, categorization and experience Acta Psychologica. 69: 231-248. DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(88)90034-0 |
0.316 |
|
1988 |
Vandierendonck A. Typically gradient in well-defined artificial categories Acta Psychologica. 69: 61-81. DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(88)90030-3 |
0.324 |
|
1988 |
Vandierendonck A, Van Damme R. Schema anticipation in recall: memory process or report strategy? Psychological Research. 50: 116-122. DOI: 10.1007/Bf00309211 |
0.336 |
|
1985 |
Schuurmans E, Vandierendonck A. Recall as communication: Effects of frame anticipation Psychological Research. 47: 119-124. DOI: 10.1007/Bf00309126 |
0.341 |
|
1984 |
Vandierendonck A. Concept learning, memory and transfer of dot pattern categories. Acta Psychologica. 55: 71-88. PMID 6711353 DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(84)90060-X |
0.323 |
|
1978 |
Vandierendonck A. Interaction of Cue Saliency, Stimulus Unitization and Familiarization in Concept Identification : Attention or Memory Phenomenon? Psychologica Belgica. 18: 224. DOI: 10.5334/Pb.634 |
0.348 |
|
1977 |
Vandierendonck A. The relation between cue salience and learning rate in concept identification. A test of the Trabasso-Bower model Acta Psychologica. 41: 467-479. DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(77)90005-1 |
0.373 |
|
Show low-probability matches. |