Matthew Heath - Publications

Affiliations: 
Kinesiology University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada 

126 high-probability publications. We are testing a new system for linking publications to authors. You can help! If you notice any inaccuracies, please sign in and mark papers as correct or incorrect matches. If you identify any major omissions or other inaccuracies in the publication list, please let us know.

Year Citation  Score
2021 Heath M, Ayala N, Hamidi M, Tari B. Distinct visual resolution supports aperture shaping in natural and pantomime-grasping. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale. PMID 34694839 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000264  0.397
2020 Hamidi M, Giuffre L, Heath M. A summary statistical representation influences perceptions but not visually or memory-guided grasping. Human Movement Science. 75: 102739. PMID 33310378 DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2020.102739  0.379
2020 Tari B, Tremblay L, Heath M. Visually guided saccades and acoustic distractors: no evidence for the remote distractor effect or global effect. Experimental Brain Research. PMID 33098653 DOI: 10.1007/s00221-020-05959-9  0.338
2020 Heath M, Shukla D. A Single Bout of Aerobic Exercise Provides an Immediate "Boost" to Cognitive Flexibility. Frontiers in Psychology. 11: 1106. PMID 32547460 DOI: 10.3389/Fpsyg.2020.01106  0.345
2020 Ayala N, Heath M. Executive Dysfunction Following a Sport-Related Concussion is Independent of Task-Based Symptom Burden. Journal of Neurotrauma. PMID 32438897 DOI: 10.1089/Neu.2019.6865  0.354
2020 Colino FL, Heath M, Hassall CD, Krigolson OE. Electroencephalographic Evidence for a Reinforcement Learning Advantage: During Motor Skill Acquisition. Biological Psychology. 107849. PMID 31981584 DOI: 10.1016/J.Biopsycho.2020.107849  0.315
2019 Tari B, Heath M. Pro- and antisaccade task-switching: response suppression-and not vector inversion-contributes to a task-set inertia. Experimental Brain Research. PMID 31741001 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-019-05686-W  0.404
2019 Pecora M, Tremblay L, Heath M. Antipointing Reaches Do Not Adhere to Width-Based Manipulations of Fitts' (1954) Equation. Motor Control. 1-16. PMID 31693993 DOI: 10.1123/Mc.2019-0010  0.45
2019 Tari B, Fadel MA, Heath M. Response suppression produces a switch-cost for spatially compatible saccades. Experimental Brain Research. PMID 30809706 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-019-05497-Z  0.424
2018 Heath M, Petrella A, Blazevic J, Lim D, Pelletier A, Belfry G. A Post-Exercise Facilitation of Executive Function is Independent of Aerobically Supported Metabolic Costs. Neuropsychologia. PMID 30321613 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2018.10.002  0.311
2018 Campbell J, Rossit S, Heath M. No Vertical Visual Field Asymmetry in Online Control: Evidence from Reaching in Depth. Motor Control. 1-18. PMID 30208756 DOI: 10.1123/Mc.2017-0095  0.481
2018 Webb B, Humphreys D, Heath M. Oculomotor Executive Dysfunction during the Early and Later Stages of Sport-Related Concussion Recovery. Journal of Neurotrauma. 35: 1874-1881. PMID 30074868 DOI: 10.1089/Neu.2018.5673  0.346
2018 Ayala N, Binsted G, Heath M. Hand anthropometry and the limits of aperture separation determine the utility of Weber's law in grasping and manual estimation. Experimental Brain Research. PMID 29923096 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-018-5311-6  0.407
2018 Manzone J, Heath M. Goal-directed reaching: the allocentric coding of target location renders an offline mode of control. Experimental Brain Research. PMID 29453490 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-018-5205-7  0.511
2018 Samani A, Heath M. Executive-related oculomotor control is improved following a 10-min single-bout of aerobic exercise: Evidence from the antisaccade task. Neuropsychologia. 108: 73-81. PMID 29191783 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2017.11.029  0.331
2017 Heath M, Chan J, Davarpanah Jazi S. Tactile-Based Pantomime Grasping: Knowledge of Results is Not Enough to Support an Absolute Calibration. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1-9. PMID 29236589 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2017.1408559  0.451
2017 Heath M, Colino FL, Chan J, Krigolson OE. Visuomotor mental rotation of a saccade: The contingent negative variation scales to the angle of rotation. Vision Research. PMID 29224981 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2017.09.005  0.377
2017 Heath M, Manzone J, Khan M, Davarpanah Jazi S. Vision for action and perception elicit dissociable adherence to Weber's law across a range of 'graspable' target objects. Experimental Brain Research. PMID 28721517 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-017-5025-1  0.412
2017 Heath M, Tremblay L, Elliott D. Fitts' Theorem and Movement Time Dissociation for Amplitude and Width Manipulations: Replying to Hoffmann. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1-3. PMID 28350243 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2017.1283290  0.401
2017 Davarpanah Jazi S, Heath M. The spatial relations between stimulus and response determine an absolute visuo-haptic calibration in pantomime-grasping. Brain and Cognition. 114: 29-39. PMID 28346879 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandc.2017.03.002  0.488
2017 Heath M, Manzone J. Manual estimations of functionally graspable target objects adhere to Weber's law. Experimental Brain Research. 235: 1701-1707. PMID 28280878 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-017-4913-8  0.414
2017 Chan J, Heath M. Haptic feedback attenuates illusory bias in pantomime-grasping: evidence for a visuo-haptic calibration. Experimental Brain Research. PMID 28070622 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-016-4860-9  0.458
2017 Manzone J, Davarpanah Jazi S, Whitwell RL, Heath M. Biomechanical constraints do not influence pantomime-grasping adherence to Weber's law: A reply to Utz et al. (2015). Vision Research. 130: 31-35. PMID 27876512 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2016.09.018  0.437
2016 Heath M, Shellington E, Titheridge S, Gill DP, Petrella RJ. A 24-Week Multi-Modality Exercise Program Improves Executive Control in Older Adults with a Self-Reported Cognitive Complaint: Evidence from the Antisaccade Task. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease : Jad. PMID 27911298 DOI: 10.3233/Jad-160627  0.321
2016 Heath M, Weiler J, Gregory MA, Gill DP, Petrella RJ. A Six-Month Cognitive-Motor and Aerobic Exercise Program Improves Executive Function in Persons with an Objective Cognitive Impairment: A Pilot Investigation Using the Antisaccade Task. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease : Jad. PMID 27567829 DOI: 10.3233/Jad-160288  0.342
2016 Heath M, DeSimone JC. The visual properties of proximal and remote distractors differentially influence reaching planning times: evidence from pro- and antipointing tasks. Experimental Brain Research. 234: 3259-3268. PMID 27405998 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-016-4723-4  0.49
2016 Heath M, Samani A, Tremblay L, Elliott D. Fitts' Theorem in Oculomotor Control: Dissociable Movement Times for Amplitude and Width Manipulations. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1-11. PMID 27341329 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2015.1134436  0.401
2016 Davarpanah Jazi S, Heath M. Pantomime-Grasping: Advance Knowledge of Haptic Feedback Availability Supports an Absolute Visuo-Haptic Calibration. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 10: 197. PMID 27199718 DOI: 10.3389/Fnhum.2016.00197  0.389
2016 Heath M, Gillen C, Samani A. Alternating between pro- and antisaccades: switch-costs manifest via decoupling the spatial relations between stimulus and response. Experimental Brain Research. 234: 853-65. PMID 26661337 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-015-4510-7  0.388
2015 Hosang S, Chan J, Davarpanah Jazi S, Heath M. Grasping a 2D object: terminal haptic feedback supports an absolute visuo-haptic calibration. Experimental Brain Research. PMID 26680769 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-015-4521-4  0.401
2015 Heath M, Hassall CD, MacLean S, Krigolson OE. Event-related brain potentials during the visuomotor mental rotation task: The contingent negative variation scales to angle of rotation. Neuroscience. 311: 153-165. PMID 26477986 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuroscience.2015.10.018  0.41
2015 de Grosbois J, Heath M, Tremblay L. Augmented feedback influences upper limb reaching movement times but does not explain violations of Fitts' Law. Frontiers in Psychology. 6: 800. PMID 26136703 DOI: 10.3389/Fpsyg.2015.00800  0.451
2015 Heath M, Gillen C, Weiler J. The antisaccade task: Vector inversion contributes to a statistical summary representation of target eccentricities. Journal of Vision. 15: 4. PMID 26053143 DOI: 10.1167/15.4.4  0.48
2015 Davarpanah Jazi S, Yau M, Westwood DA, Heath M. Pantomime-grasping: the 'return' of haptic feedback supports the absolute specification of object size. Experimental Brain Research. 233: 2029-40. PMID 25869741 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-015-4274-0  0.455
2015 DeSimone JC, Everling S, Heath M. The antisaccade task: visual distractors elicit a location-independent planning 'cost'. Plos One. 10: e0122345. PMID 25830383 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0122345  0.468
2015 Davarpanah Jazi S, Hosang S, Heath M. Memory delay and haptic feedback influence the dissociation of tactile cues for perception and action Neuropsychologia. 71: 91-100. PMID 25796409 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2015.03.018  0.461
2015 Heath M, Starrs F, Macpherson E, Weiler J. Task-switching effects for visual and auditory pro- and antisaccades: evidence for a task-set inertia. Journal of Motor Behavior. 47: 319-27. PMID 25584657 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2014.982068  0.429
2015 Weiler J, Hassall CD, Krigolson OE, Heath M. The unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost: electroencephalographic evidence of task-set inertia in oculomotor control. Behavioural Brain Research. 278: 323-9. PMID 25453741 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bbr.2014.10.012  0.328
2015 Heath M, Starrs F, MacPherson E, Weiler J. Task-switching effects for visual and auditory pro-and antisaccades: Evidence for a task-set inertia Journal of Motor Behavior. 47: 319-327. DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2014.982068  0.312
2014 Gillen C, Heath M. Target frequency influences antisaccade endpoint bias: evidence for perceptual averaging. Vision Research. 105: 151-8. PMID 25449162 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2014.10.010  0.457
2014 Weiler J, Heath M. Oculomotor task switching: Alternating from a nonstandard to a standard response yields the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost Journal of Neurophysiology. 112: 2176-2184. PMID 25122700 DOI: 10.1152/Jn.00352.2014  0.357
2014 Gillen C, Heath M. Perceptual averaging governs antisaccade endpoint bias. Experimental Brain Research. 232: 3201-10. PMID 24935477 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-014-4010-1  0.418
2014 Davarpanah Jazi S, Heath M. Weber's law in tactile grasping and manual estimation: Feedback-dependent evidence for functionally distinct processing streams Brain and Cognition. 86: 32-41. PMID 24556320 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandc.2014.01.014  0.399
2014 Weiler J, Mitchell T, Heath M. Response suppression delays the planning of subsequent stimulus-driven saccades. Plos One. 9: e86408. PMID 24466076 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0086408  0.4
2014 Weiler J, Heath M. Repetitive antisaccade execution does not increase the unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost Acta Psychologica. 146: 67-72. PMID 24412836 DOI: 10.1016/J.Actpsy.2013.12.005  0.426
2014 DeSimone JC, Weiler J, Aber GS, Heath M. The unidirectional prosaccade switch-cost: Correct and error antisaccades differentially influence the planning times for subsequent prosaccades Vision Research. 96: 17-24. PMID 24412739 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2013.12.005  0.377
2014 Gillen C, Diamond J, Heath M. The asymmetrical weighting of target eccentricities within a trial block influences antisaccade endpoint bias Journal of Vision. 14: 95-95. DOI: 10.1167/14.10.95  0.305
2014 DeSimone J, Aber G, Heath M. The antisaccade task: Sensory- and motor-related costs to oculomotor planning Journal of Vision. 14: 94-94. DOI: 10.1167/14.10.94  0.442
2013 Holmes SA, Lohmus J, McKinnon S, Mulla A, Heath M. Distinct visual cues mediate aperture shaping for grasping and pantomime-grasping tasks. Journal of Motor Behavior. 45: 431-9. PMID 23971991 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2013.818930  0.498
2013 Gillen C, Weiler J, Heath M. Stimulus-driven saccades are characterized by an invariant undershooting bias: no evidence for a range effect. Experimental Brain Research. 230: 165-74. PMID 23884554 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-013-3640-Z  0.415
2013 Holmes SA, Heath M. Goal-directed grasping: the dimensional properties of an object influence the nature of the visual information mediating aperture shaping. Brain and Cognition. 82: 18-24. PMID 23501700 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandc.2013.02.005  0.379
2013 Gillen C, Weiler J, Heath M. Target range properties do not influence oculomotor undershooting bias. Journal of Vision. 13: 1222-1222. DOI: 10.1167/13.9.1222  0.3
2012 Heath M, Holmes SA, Mulla A, Binsted G. Grasping time does not influence the early adherence of aperture shaping to Weber's law. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 6: 332. PMID 23267323 DOI: 10.3389/Fnhum.2012.00332  0.404
2012 Weiler J, Heath M. Task-switching in oculomotor control: Unidirectional switch-cost when alternating between pro- and antisaccades Neuroscience Letters. 530: 150-154. PMID 23063688 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neulet.2012.10.007  0.38
2012 Heath M, Bell J, Holroyd CB, Krigolson O. Electroencephalographic evidence of vector inversion in antipointing. Experimental Brain Research. 221: 19-26. PMID 22710619 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-012-3141-5  0.467
2012 Krigolson O, Bell J, Kent CM, Heath M, Holroyd CB. Reduced cortical motor potentials underlie reductions in memory-guided reaching performance. Motor Control. 16: 353-70. PMID 22643098 DOI: 10.1123/Mcj.16.3.353  0.389
2012 Weiler J, Heath M. The prior-antisaccade effect influences the planning and online control of prosaccades. Experimental Brain Research. 216: 545-52. PMID 22120158 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-011-2958-7  0.422
2012 Weiler J, Heath M. The prior-antisaccade effect: Decoupling stimulus and response inhibits the planning and control of subsequent prosaccades Journal of Vision. 12: 1253-1253. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.1253  0.301
2012 Holmes S, Marriott K, Mackenzie A, Sin M, Heath M. Distinct visual metrics support the late stages of aperture shaping for 2D and 3D target objects Journal of Vision. 12: 1096-1096. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.1096  0.326
2012 Marriott K, Holmes S, Tay J, Heath M. Goal-directed grasping: Visual and haptic percepts of object size influence early but not late aperture shaping Journal of Vision. 12: 1095-1095. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.1095  0.387
2012 Heath M, Bell J, Holroyd C, Krigolson O. Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence of visual vector inversion in antipointing Journal of Vision. 12: 1086-1086. DOI: 10.1167/12.9.1086  0.356
2011 Neely KA, Heath M. The visuomotor mental rotation task: visuomotor transformation times are reduced for small and perceptually familiar angles. Journal of Motor Behavior. 43: 393-402. PMID 21978212 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2011.618850  0.73
2011 Cheng DT, De Grosbois J, Smirl J, Heath M, Binsted G. Preceding movement effects on sequential aiming. Experimental Brain Research. 215: 1-11. PMID 21947132 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-011-2862-1  0.464
2011 Bingley T, Heath M. Goal-directed reaching: the SNARC effect influences movement planning but not movement execution. Seeing and Perceiving. 24: 223-40. PMID 21864464 DOI: 10.1163/187847511X576794  0.446
2011 Weiler J, Holmes SA, Mulla A, Heath M. Pro- and antisaccades: dissociating stimulus and response influences the online control of saccade trajectories. Journal of Motor Behavior. 43: 375-81. PMID 21861629 DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2011.604656  0.428
2011 Holmes SA, Mulla A, Binsted G, Heath M. Visually and memory-guided grasping: aperture shaping exhibits a time-dependent scaling to Weber's law. Vision Research. 51: 1941-8. PMID 21777599 DOI: 10.1016/J.Visres.2011.07.005  0.445
2011 Heath M, Weiler J, Marriott KA, Elliott D, Binsted G. Revisiting Fitts and Peterson (1964): width and amplitude manipulations to the reaching environment elicit dissociable movement times. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne De Psychologie ExpéRimentale. 65: 259-68. PMID 21728407 DOI: 10.1037/A0023618  0.413
2011 Heath M, Weiler J, Marriott K, Welsh TN. Vector inversion diminishes the online control of antisaccades. Experimental Brain Research. 209: 117-27. PMID 21210087 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-010-2525-7  0.432
2011 Heath M, Mulla A, Holmes SA, Smuskowitz LR. The visual coding of grip aperture shows an early but not late adherence to Weber's law. Neuroscience Letters. 490: 200-4. PMID 21194553 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neulet.2010.12.051  0.472
2011 Weiler J, Holmes S, Mulla A, Heath M. Distinct Response Latencies do not Influence Pro- and Antisaccade Trajectories Journal of Vision. 11: 550-550. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.550  0.301
2011 Heath M, Weiler J, Marriott K, Welsh T. The antisaccade task: dissociating stimulus and response influences online saccade control Journal of Vision. 11: 549-549. DOI: 10.1167/11.11.549  0.381
2010 Neely KA, Heath M. Visuomotor mental rotation: reaction time is determined by the complexity of the sensorimotor transformations mediating the response. Brain Research. 1366: 129-40. PMID 20920488 DOI: 10.1016/J.Brainres.2010.09.096  0.672
2010 Heath M, Dunham K, Binsted G, Godbolt B. Antisaccades exhibit diminished online control relative to prosaccades Experimental Brain Research. 203: 743-752. PMID 20490784 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-010-2290-7  0.447
2010 Brownell K, Rolheiser T, Heath M, Binsted G. Does perception asymmetrically influence motor production in upper and lower visual fields? Motor Control. 14: 44-58. PMID 20237402 DOI: 10.1123/Mcj.14.1.44  0.474
2010 Maraj A, Heath M. Antipointing: perception-based visual information renders an offline mode of control. Experimental Brain Research. 202: 55-64. PMID 20012599 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-009-2111-Z  0.449
2010 Neely KA, Heath M. Visuomotor mental rotation: the reaction time advantage for anti-pointing is not influenced by perceptual experience with the cardinal axes. Experimental Brain Research. 201: 593-8. PMID 19882148 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-009-2056-2  0.725
2010 Heath M, Maraj A, Binsted G. The anti-pointing task: Vector inversion is mediated by a perceptual representation of reaching space Journal of Vision. 9: 1101-1101. DOI: 10.1167/9.8.1101  0.326
2010 Heath M, Neely K, Yakimishyn J, Binsted G. Visuomotor performance and visuomotor memory operate without conscious awareness of intrinsic target features Journal of Vision. 8: 613-613. DOI: 10.1167/8.6.613  0.66
2010 Heath M, Tremblay L, Binsted G. Vision predominates sensorimotor transformations for online grasping control Journal of Vision. 7: 161-161. DOI: 10.1167/7.9.161  0.307
2010 Neely K, Heath M. Visuomotor mental rotation: Reaction time is determined by the complexity of sensorimotor transformations supporting the response Journal of Vision. 10: 1072-1072. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.1072  0.664
2010 de Grosbois J, Colino F, Krigolson O, Heath M, Binsted G. EEG microstates during visually guided reaching Journal of Vision. 10: 1068-1068. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.1068  0.356
2010 Bingley T, Heath M. Digit magnitude does not influence the spatial parameters of goal-directed reaching movements Journal of Vision. 10: 1061-1061. DOI: 10.1167/10.7.1061  0.358
2009 Neely KA, Heath M. Visuomotor mental rotation: reaction time is not a function of the angle of rotation. Neuroscience Letters. 463: 194-8. PMID 19632298 DOI: 10.1016/J.Neulet.2009.07.060  0.733
2009 Heath M, Maraj A, Maddigan M, Binsted G. The antipointing task: vector inversion is supported by a perceptual estimate of visual space. Journal of Motor Behavior. 41: 383-92. PMID 19460747 DOI: 10.3200/35-08-016  0.5
2009 Heath M, Maraj A, Gradkowski A, Binsted G. Anti-pointing is mediated by a perceptual bias of target location in left and right visual space. Experimental Brain Research. 192: 275-86. PMID 18982320 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-008-1612-5  0.451
2008 Heath M, Maraj A, Godbolt B, Binsted G. Action without awareness: reaching to an object you do not remember seeing. Plos One. 3: e3539. PMID 18953411 DOI: 10.1371/Journal.Pone.0003539  0.479
2008 Heath M, Neely K, Krigolson O. Response modes influence the accuracy of monocular and binocular reaching movements. Motor Control. 12: 252-66. PMID 18698109 DOI: 10.1123/Mcj.12.3.252  0.755
2008 Neely KA, Heath M, Binsted G. Egocentric and allocentric visual cues influence the specification of movement distance and direction. Journal of Motor Behavior. 40: 203-13. PMID 18477534 DOI: 10.3200/Jmbr.40.3.203-213  0.747
2008 Heath M, Neely KA, Yakimishyn J, Binsted G. Visuomotor memory is independent of conscious awareness of target features. Experimental Brain Research. 188: 517-27. PMID 18443770 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-008-1385-X  0.737
2008 Ehresman C, Saucier D, Heath M, Binsted G. Online corrections can produce illusory bias during closed-loop pointing. Experimental Brain Research. 188: 371-8. PMID 18427794 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-008-1367-Z  0.462
2008 Neely KA, Tessmer A, Binsted G, Heath M. Goal-directed reaching: movement strategies influence the weighting of allocentric and egocentric visual cues. Experimental Brain Research. 186: 375-84. PMID 18087697 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-007-1238-Z  0.754
2008 Heath M, Grierson L, Binsted G, Elliott D. Interhemispheric transmission time in persons with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research : Jidr. 51: 972-81. PMID 17991004 DOI: 10.1111/J.1365-2788.2007.01009.X  0.329
2008 Heath M. Visual information and the online control of action: A test of duplex visual processing Brain and Cognition. 67: 2-3. DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandc.2008.02.010  0.375
2008 Maraj BKV, Robertson SD, Welsh TN, Weeks DJ, Chua R, Heath M, Roy EA, Simon DA, Weinberg H, Elliott D. Verbal-Motor Behaviour in Persons with Down Syndrome Down Syndrome Across the Life Span. 175-193. DOI: 10.1002/9780470777886.ch14  0.37
2007 Binsted G, Brownell K, Vorontsova Z, Heath M, Saucier D. Visuomotor system uses target features unavailable to conscious awareness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 104: 12669-72. PMID 17646661 DOI: 10.1073/Pnas.0702307104  0.468
2007 Krigolson O, Clark N, Heath M, Binsted G. The proximity of visual landmarks impacts reaching performance. Spatial Vision. 20: 317-36. PMID 17594798 DOI: 10.1163/156856807780919028  0.479
2007 Heath M, Binsted G. Visuomotor memory for target location in near and far reaching spaces. Journal of Motor Behavior. 39: 169-77. PMID 17550869 DOI: 10.3200/Jmbr.39.3.169-178  0.401
2007 Heath M, Neely K, Binsted G. Allocentric visual cues influence online limb adjustments. Motor Control. 11: 54-70. PMID 17392567  0.744
2006 Krigolson O, Van Gyn G, Tremblay L, Heath M. Is there "feedback" during visual imagery? Evidence from a specificity of practice paradigm. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne De Psychologie ExpéRimentale. 60: 24-32. PMID 16615715 DOI: 10.1037/Cjep2006004  0.43
2006 Krigolson O, Heath M. A lower visual field advantage for endpoint stability but no advantage for online movement precision. Experimental Brain Research. 170: 127-35. PMID 16501960 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-006-0386-X  0.5
2006 Heath M, Rival C, Neely K, Krigolson O. Müller-Lyer figures influence the online reorganization of visually guided grasping movements. Experimental Brain Research. 169: 473-81. PMID 16292638 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-005-0170-3  0.736
2006 Heath M, Rival C, Neely K. Visual feedback schedules influence visuomotor resistance to the Müller-Lyer figures. Experimental Brain Research. 168: 348-56. PMID 16175361 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-005-0095-X  0.738
2005 Heath M. Role of limb and target vision in the online control of memory-guided reaches Motor Control. 9: 281-309. PMID 16239717 DOI: 10.1123/Mcj.9.3.281  0.454
2005 Heath M, Rival C, Westwood DA, Neely K. Time course analysis of closed- and open-loop grasping of the Müller-Lyer illusion. Journal of Motor Behavior. 37: 179-85. PMID 15883115 DOI: 10.3200/Jmbr.37.3.179-185  0.736
2005 Westwood DA, Robertson C, Heath M. Action control: independent effects of memory and monocular viewing on reaching accuracy. Brain and Cognition. 57: 257-60. PMID 15780459 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandc.2004.08.055  0.398
2005 Heath M, Rival C. Role of the visuomotor system in on-line attenuation of a premovement illusory bias in grip aperture. Brain and Cognition. 57: 111-4. PMID 15708199 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandc.2004.08.029  0.364
2005 Heath M, Welsh TN, Simon DA, Tremblay L, Elliott D, Roy EA. Relative processing demands influence cerebral laterality for verbal-motor integration in persons with Down syndrome. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 41: 61-6. PMID 15633707 DOI: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70178-3  0.534
2005 Neely K, Binsted G, Heath M. Manual asymmetries in bimanual reaching: the influence of spatial compatibility and visuospatial attention. Brain and Cognition. 57: 102-5. PMID 15629221 DOI: 10.1016/J.Bandc.2004.08.041  0.729
2005 Binsted G, Heath M. No evidence of a lower visual field specialization for visuomotor control. Experimental Brain Research. 162: 89-94. PMID 15517212 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-004-2108-6  0.497
2004 Krigolson O, Heath M. Background visual cues and memory-guided reaching. Human Movement Science. 23: 861-77. PMID 15664677 DOI: 10.1016/J.Humov.2004.10.011  0.511
2004 Heath M, Rival C, Binsted G. Can the motor system resolve a premovement bias in grip aperture? Online analysis of grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion. Experimental Brain Research. 158: 378-84. PMID 15278330 DOI: 10.1007/S00221-004-1988-9  0.467
2004 Heath M, Westwood DA, Binsted G. The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space. Motor Control. 8: 76-106. PMID 14973339 DOI: 10.1123/Mcj.8.1.76  0.481
2004 Binsted G, Heath M. Can the motor system utilize a stored representation to control movement Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 27: 25-27. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X0424002X  0.434
2003 Heath M, Westwood DA. Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evident from a variable spatial mapping paradigm. Motor Control. 7: 346-61. PMID 14999133 DOI: 10.1123/Mcj.7.4.349  0.496
2003 Heath M, Almeida QJ, Roy EA, Black SE, Westwood D. Selective dysfunction of tool-use: a failure to integrate somatosensation and action. Neurocase. 9: 156-63. PMID 12925939 DOI: 10.1076/Neur.9.2.156.15072  0.534
2003 Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA. No evidence for accurate visuomotor memory: systematic and variable error in memory-guided reaching. Journal of Motor Behavior. 35: 127-33. PMID 12711584 DOI: 10.1080/00222890309602128  0.567
2002 Heath M, Westwood DA, Roy EA, Young RP. Manual asymmetries in tool-use: implications for apraxia. Laterality. 7: 131-43. PMID 15513193 DOI: 10.1080/13576500143000168  0.538
2001 Heath M, Roy EA, Black SE, Westwood DA. Intransitive limb gestures and apraxia following unilateral stroke. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 23: 628-42. PMID 11778640 DOI: 10.1076/Jcen.23.5.628.1240  0.491
2001 Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA. The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expã©Rimentale. 55: 304-10. PMID 11768855 DOI: 10.1037/H0087377  0.557
2001 Heath M, Roy EA, Westwood D, Black SE. Patterns of apraxia associated with the production of intransitive limb gestures following left and right hemisphere stroke. Brain and Cognition. 46: 165-9. PMID 11527320 DOI: 10.1016/S0278-2626(01)80057-9  0.455
2000 Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA. The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension. Experimental Brain Research. 134: 456-63. PMID 11081827 DOI: 10.1007/S002210000489  0.625
2000 Roy EA, Heath M, Westwood D, Schweizer TA, Dixon MJ, Black SE, Kalbfleisch L, Barbour K, Square PA. Task demands and limb apraxia in stroke. Brain and Cognition. 44: 253-79. PMID 11041991 DOI: 10.1006/Brcg.2000.1230  0.457
1999 Elliott D, Heath M, Binsted G, Ricker KL, Roy EA, Chua R. Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. Journal of Motor Behavior. 31: 309-324. PMID 11177640 DOI: 10.1080/00222899909600997  0.581
1999 Heath M, Roy EA, Weir PL. Visual-Motor Integration of Unexpected Sensory Events in Young and Older Participants: A Kinematic Analysis Developmental Neuropsychology. 16: 197-211. DOI: 10.1207/S15326942Dn1602_4  0.588
1999 Elliott D, Binsted G, Heath M. The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory Human Movement Science. 18: 121-136. DOI: 10.1016/S0167-9457(99)00004-4  0.466
1998 Heath M, Hodges NJ, Chua R, Elliott D. On-line control of rapid aiming movements: Unexpected target perturbations and movement kinematics Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 52: 163-173. DOI: 10.1037/H0087289  0.461
Show low-probability matches.